Assessment |
Biopsychology |
Comparative |
Cognitive |
Developmental |
Language |
Individual differences |
Personality |
Philosophy |
Social |
Methods |
Statistics |
Clinical |
Educational |
Industrial |
Professional items |
World psychology |
Animals · Animal ethology · Comparative psychology · Animal models · Outline · Index
Animal cognition, is the title given to a modern approach to the mental capacities of animals other than humans. It has developed out of comparative psychology, but has also been strongly influenced by the approach of ethology, behavioral ecology, and evolutionary psychology. The alternative name cognitive ethology is therefore sometimes used; and much of what used to be considered under the title of animal intelligence is now thought of under this heading. Note that in this article, as in the field of animal cognition, "animal" is used here and in the following in the usual sense of "non-human animal", even though its meaning in biology normally comprises all members of the kingdom Animalia, including humans.
Research in animal cognition mostly concerns mammals, especially primates, cetaceans, and elephants, as well as dogs, cats, raccoons and rodents. However, research also extends to non-mammalian vertebrates such as birds including parrots, corvids, and pigeons, as well as to reptiles such as lizards and snakes, and to fish, even to invertebrates such as cephalopods, spiders, and insects.[1]
Animal groups studied[]
The animal groups studied include:
- Bird intelligence
- Cat intelligence
- Cephalopod intelligence
- Cetacean intelligence
- Chimpanzee intelligence
- Dog intelligence
- Elephant cognition
- Fish intelligence
- Pigeon intelligence
- Primate cognition
Historical background[]
Animal cognition from anecdote to laboratory[]
The behavior of non-human animals has captivated human imagination from antiquity, and over the centuries many writers have speculated about the animal mind, or its absence, as Descartes would have it.[2] Speculation about animal intelligence gradually yielded to scientific study after Darwin placed humans and animals on a continuum, although Darwin’s largely anecdotal approach to the topic would not pass scientific muster later on.[3] Unsatisfied with the anecdotal method of Darwin and his protégé Romanes,[4] E. L. Thorndike brought animal behavior into the laboratory for objective scrutiny. Thorndike’s careful observations of the escape of cats, dogs, and chicks from puzzle boxes led him to conclude that intelligent behavior may be compounded of simple associations and that inference to animal reason, insight, or consciousness is unnecessary and misleading.[5] At about the same time, I. P. Pavlov began his seminal studies of conditioned reflexes in dogs. Pavlov quickly abandoned attempts to infer canine mental processes; such attempts, he said, led only to disagreement and confusion. He was, however, willing to propose unseen physiological processes that might explain his observations.[6]
The behavioristic half-century[]
For most of the twentieth century, the dominant approach to animal psychology was to use experiments on intelligence in animals to uncover simple processes (such as classical conditioning and operant conditioning) that might then account for the apparently more complex intellectual abilities of humans. This approach is well summarised in the mid-century book by Hilgard (1958), but its reductionist philosophy was combined with a strongly behaviorist methodology, in which overt behavior was taken as the only valid data for the study of psychology, and in its more extreme forms (the radical behaviorism of B. F. Skinner and his experimental analysis of behavior) behavior was taken as the only topic of interest. In effect, the mental processes that humans experience in themselves were viewed as epiphenomena (see, for example, Skinner, 1969). The work of Thorndike, Pavlov and a little later of the outspoken behaviorist John B. Watson[7] set the direction of much research on animal behavior for more than half a century. During this time there was considerable progress in understanding simple associations; notably, around 1930 the differences between Thorndike's instrumental (or operant) conditioning and Pavlov's classical (or Pavlovian) conditioning were clarified, first by Miller and Kanorski, and then by B. F. Skinner.[8][9] Many experiments on conditioning followed; they generated some complex theories,[10] but they made little or no reference to intervening mental processes. Probably the most explicit dismissal of the idea that mental processes control behavior was the radical behaviorism of Skinner. This view seeks to explain behavior, including "private events" like mental images, solely by reference to the environmental contingencies impinging on the human or animal.[11]
Despite the predominantly behaviorist orientation of research before 1960, the rejection of mental processes in animals was not universal during those years. Influential exceptions included, for example, Wolfgang Köhler and his insightful chimpanzees[12] and Edward Tolman whose proposed cognitive map was a significant contribution to subsequent cognitive research in both humans and animals.[13]
The cognitive revolution[]
The success of cognitive psychology in addressing human mental processes, which began in the late 1950s and was proclaimed by Neisser (1967), led to a re-evaluation of the research paradigm, and researchers began to address animal mental processes from the opposite direction, by taking what is known about human mental processes and looking for evidence of comparable processes in other species. In a sense this was a return to the approach of Darwin's protegé George Romanes (e.g. 1886), arguably the first comparative psychologist of the modern era. However, whereas Romanes relied heavily on anecdote and an anthropomorphic projection of human capacities onto other species, modern researchers in animal cognition are in most cases firmly behaviorist in methodology, even though they differ sharply from the behaviorist philosophy. There are some exceptions to the rule of behaviorist methodology, such as John Lilly and, some would argue, Donald Griffin (e.g. 1992), who have been prepared to take a strong position that other animals do have minds and that humans should approach the study of their cognition accordingly. However, their claims have not found wide acceptance in the scientific community, though they have attracted an enthusiastic following among lay people.
Beginning around 1960, a "cognitive revolution" in research on humans[14] gradually spurred a similar transformation of research with animals. Inference to processes not directly observable became acceptable and then commonplace. An important proponent of this shift in thinking was Donald O. Hebb, who argued that "mind" is simply a name for processes in the head that control complex behavior, and that it is both necessary and possible to infer those processes from behavior.[15] Animals came to be seen as "goal seeking agents that acquire, store, retrieve, and internally process information at many levels of cognitive complexity.".[16] However, it is interesting to note that many cognitive experiments with animals made, and still make, ingenious use of conditioning methods pioneered by Thorndike and Pavlov.[17]
The development of animal cognition was also strongly influenced by:
- increased use of and interest in primates (and also cetaceans) rather than the rats and pigeons that had become the classic species of the comparative psychology laboratory, and by developments within primatology;
- advancing knowledge of animals' behavior in their natural environments through studies in ethology, sociobiology and behavioral ecology; such studies often showed that animals needed certain cognitive abilities in order to adapt to their ecological niche (as for example in studies of caching birds such as Clark's Nutcracker by Alan Kamil and his colleagues (e.g. Kamil & Balda, 1990), or appeared to use cognitive abilities under natural conditions (for example in Jane Goodall's studies of chimpanzees, see Goodall (1991);
- one or two high profile projects, in particular Allen and Beatrice Gardner's Washoe project in which a chimpanzee learned at least some elements of American Sign Language.
- advancing understanding of brain function through work in physiological psychology and cognitive neuropsychology
This account of the history of the study of animal cognition is inevitably oversimplified. From Romanes on, there have always been comparative psychologists who have been more or less cognitively inclined: obvious examples are Wolfgang Köhler, famous for his studies of insight in chimpanzees, and Edward C. Tolman, who introduced into psychology, as an explanation of the behavior of rats in mazes, two ideas that have been immensely influential in human cognitive psychology - the cognitive map and the idea of decision-making in risky choice according to expected value.
Methodology[]
The acceleration of research on animal cognition in the last 50 years has led to a rapid expansion in the variety of species studied and methods employed. The remarkable behavior of large-brained animals such as primates and cetacea has claimed special attention, but all sorts of mammals large and small, birds, fish, ants, bees, and others have been brought into the laboratory or observed in carefully controlled field studies. In the laboratory, animals push levers, pull strings, dig for food, swim in water mazes, or respond to images on computer screens in discrimination, attention, memory, and categorization experiments.[17] Careful field studies explore memory for food caches, navigation by stars,[citation needed] communication, tool use, identification of conspecifics, and many other matters. Studies often focus on the behavior of animals in their natural environments and discuss the putative function of the behavior for the propagation and survival of the species. These developments reflect an increased cross-fertilization from related fields such as ethology and behavioral biology. Also, contributions from behavioral neuroscience are beginning to clarify the physiological substrate of some inferred mental process.
Research in animal cognition continues to use some of the established research techniques of comparative psychology and the experimental analysis of behavior, such as mazes and Skinner boxes, though it employs them in new varieties (such as the 8-arm maze and water maze that have been used in many studies of spatial memory) and in new ways. However, it complements those with observation of animals in their natural environments, or quasi-natural environments and also with field experiments. It has also been characterised by a number of very long term projects, such as the Washoe project and other ape-language experiments (e.g. project Nim), Irene Pepperberg's extended series of studies with the African Gray Parrot Alex, Louis Herman's work with bottlenosed dolphins, and studies of long-term memory in pigeons in which birds were shown to remember pictures for periods of several years. Some cognitive research also requires the management of animal behavior, and the use of operant conditioning to facilitate animal training. In general, the conclusion of concept formation in an animal requires a generalization test where the animal responds appropriately to a novel stimulus to which associative learning cannot explain the response behavior. Some researchers have made effective use of a Piagetian methodology, taking tasks which human children are known to master at different stages of development, and investigating which of them can be performed by particular species. Others have been inspired by concerns for animal welfare and the management of domestic species: for example Temple Grandin has harnessed her unique expertise in animal welfare and the ethical treatment of farm livestock to highlight underlying similarities between humans and other animals.
Several long term research projects have captured a good deal of attention. These include ape-language experiments such as the Washoe project and project Nim. Other animal projects include Irene Pepperberg's extended series of studies with the African Gray Parrot Alex, Louis Herman's work with bottlenosed dolphins, and studies of long-term memory in pigeons in which birds were shown to remember pictures for periods of several years.
Some researchers have made effective use of a Piagetian methodology, taking tasks which human children are known to master at different stages of development, and investigating which of them can be performed by particular species. Others have been inspired by concerns for animal welfare and the management of domestic species: for example Temple Grandin has harnessed her unique expertise in animal welfare and the ethical treatment of farm livestock to highlight underlying similarities between humans and other animals.[18] From a methodological point of view, one of the main risks in this sort of work is anthropomorphism, the tendency to interpret an animal's behavior in terms of human feelings, thoughts, and motivations.[1]
Research questions[]
Given the broad program of animal cognition, of looking for the animal analogs of human cognitive processes, the areas of study in animal cognition follow more or less from those in human cognitive psychology. However, progress in the different areas has been variable. Among the fields of interest are:
Attention[]
Research has focused on animals' ability to distribute attention between different aspects of a stimulus, and on visual search. As in humans, it appears that sharing attention between stimulus features reduces the capacity to detect any one of them, though there are some ecologically relevant visual search tasks at which particular species show remarkable abilities (for example, pigeons have an extraordinary capacity to pick out grain from substrate).
Much of what is happening in the world at any moment is irrelevant to current behavior. Attention refers to mental processes that select relevant information, inhibit irrelevant information, and switch among these as the situation demands.[19] Often the selective process is tuned before relevant information appears; such expectation makes for rapid selection of key stimuli when they become available. A large body of research has explored the way attention and expectation affect the behavior of non-human animals, and much of this work suggests that attention operates in birds, mammals and reptiles in much the same way that it does in humans.[20]
The following paragraphs contain brief accounts of several experiments. These are intended to give the reader a bit of the flavor of research on attention, but they barely scratch the surface, and readers should consult the references for descriptions of many other experiments. Also, one must interpret putative "attentional" effects with caution, because they can often be accounted for in several different ways. For example, lack of response to a current stimulus might reflect inattention, but it might also reflect lack of motivation, or result from past learning that suppresses response to that stimulus or promotes an alternative response. Most experiments include control conditions intended to exclude as many alternative interpretations as possible.
Selective learning[]
Animals trained to discriminate between two stimuli, say black versus white, can be said to attend to the "brightness dimension," but this says little about whether this dimension is selected in preference to others. More enlightenment comes from experiments that allow the animal to choose from several alternatives. For example, several studies have shown that performance is better on, for example, a color discrimination (e.g. blue vs green) after the animal has learned another color discrimination (e.g. red vs orange) than it is after training on a different dimension such as an X shape versus and O shape. The reverse effect happens after training on forms. Thus, the earlier learning appears to affect which dimension, color or form, the animal will attend to.[21]
Other experiments have shown that after animals have learned to respond to one aspect of the environment responsiveness to other aspects is suppressed. In "blocking", for example, an animal is conditioned to respond to one stimulus ("A") by pairing that stimulus with reward or punishment. After the animal responds consistently to A, a second stimulus ("B") accompanies A on additional training trials. Later tests with the B stimulus alone elicit little response, suggesting that learning about B has been blocked by prior learning about A .[22] This result supports the hypothesis that stimuli are neglected if they fail to provide new information. Thus, in the experiment just cited, the animal failed to attend to B because B added no information to that supplied by A. If true, this interpretation is an important insight into attentional processing, but this conclusion remains uncertain because blocking and several related phenomena can be explained by models of conditioning that do not invoke attention.[23]
Divided attention[]
Casual observation suggests that attention is a limited resource and is not all-or-none: the more attention is devoted to one aspect or dimension of the environment, the less is available for others.[24] In preparing a meal you may divide your attention among a number of things, but a sudden spill may distract you from a falling souffle. A number of experiments have studied this sort of thing in animals. For example, in one experiment, a tone and a light came on simultaneously. The pigeon subjects gained reward only by choosing the correct combination of the two dimensions (a high pitch together with a yellow light). The birds did fairly well at this task, presumably by dividing attention between the two dimensions. When only one of the stimulus dimensions varied, while the other was held at its rewarded value, discrimination improved on the variable stimulus, and later tests showed that discrimination had also gotten worse on the alternative stimulus dimension.[25] These outcomes are consistent with the idea that attention is a limited resource that can be more or less focused among incoming stimuli.
Visual search and attentional priming[]
As noted above, attention functions to select information that is of special use to the animal. Visual search typically calls for this sort of selection, and search tasks have been used extensively in both humans and animals to determine the characteristics of attentional selection and the factors that control it.
Experimental research on visual search in animals was initially prompted by field observations published by Luc Tinbergen (1960).[26] Tinbergen observed that birds are selective when foraging for insects. For example, he found that birds tended to catch the same type of insect repeatedly even though several types were available. Tinbergen suggested that this prey selection was caused by an attentional bias that improved detection of one type of insect while suppressing detection of others. This "attentional priming" is commonly said to result from a pretrial activation of a mental representation of the attended object, which Tinbergen called a "searching image."
Tinbergen’s field observations on priming have been supported by a number of experiments. For example, Pietrewicz and Kamil (1977, 1979)[27][28] presented blue jays with pictures of tree trunks upon which rested either a moth of species A, a moth of species B, or no moth at all. The birds were rewarded for pecks at a picture showing a moth. Crucially, the probability with which a particular species of moth was detected was higher after repeated trials with that species (e.g. A, A, A,...) than it was after a mixture of trials (e.g. A, B, B, A, B, A, A...). These results suggest again that sequential encounters with an object can establish an attentional predisposition to see the object.
Another way to produce attentional priming in search is to provide an advance signal that is associated with the target. For example, if you hear a song sparrow you may be predisposed to detect a song sparrow in a shrub, or among other birds. A number of experiments have reproduced this effect in animal subjects.[29][30]
Still other experiments have explored nature of stimulus factors that affect the speed and accuracy of visual search. For example, the time taken to find a single target increases as the number of items in the visual field increases. This rise in RT is steep if the distracters are similar to the target, less steep if they are dissimilar, and may not occur if the distracters are very different in from the target in form or color.[31]
Concepts and categories[]
Fundamental but difficult to define, the concept of "concept" was discussed for hundreds of years by philosophers before it became a focus of psychological study. Concepts enable humans and animals to organize the world into functional groups; the groups may be composed of perceptually similar objects or events, diverse things that have a common function, relationships such as same versus different, or relations among relations such as analogies.[32] Extensive discussions on these matters together with many references may be found in Shettleworth (2010) Wasserman and Zentall (2006)[17] and in Zentall et al. (2008). The latter is freely available online[33] Following pioneering research by Richard Herrnstein, there has been a mass of research on birds' ability to discriminate between categories of stimuli, including the kinds of ill-defined category that are used in everyday human speech. Birds have been found to learn this kind of task easily, and to transfer correct responses readily to new instances of the categories.
Methods[]
Most work on animal concepts has been done with visual stimuli, which can easily be constructed and presented in great variety, but auditory and other stimuli have been used as well.[34] Pigeons have been widely used, for they have excellent vision and are readily conditioned to respond to visual targets; other birds and a number of other animals have been studied as well.[1] In a typical experiment, a bird or other animal confronts a computer monitor on which a large number of pictures appear one by one, and the subject gets a reward for pecking or touching a picture of a category item and no reward for non-category items. Alternatively, a subject may be offered a choice between two or more pictures. Many experiments end with the presentation of items never seen before; successful sorting of these items shows that the animal has not simply learned many specific stimulus-response associations. A related method, sometimes used to study relational concepts, is matching-to-sample. In this task an animal sees one stimulus and then chooses between two or more alternatives, one of which is the same as the first; the animal is then rewarded for choosing the matching stimulus.[1][17][33]
Perceptual categories[]
Perceptual categorization is said to occur when a person or animal responds in a similar way to a range of stimuli that share common features. For example, a squirrel climbs a tree when it sees Rex, Shep, or Trixie, which suggests that it categorizes all three as something to avoid. This sorting of instances into groups is crucial to survival. Among other things, an animal must categorize if it is to apply learning about one object (e.g. Rex bit me) to new instances of that category (dogs may bite).[1][17][33]
Natural categories[]
Many animals readily classify objects by perceived differences in form or color. For example, bees or pigeons quickly learn to choose any red object and reject any green object if red leads to reward and green does not. Seemingly much more difficult is an animal’s ability to categorize natural objects that vary a great deal in color and form even while belonging to the same group. In a classic study, Richard J. Herrnstein trained pigeons to respond to the presence or absence of human beings in photographs.[35] The birds readily learned to peck photos that contained partial or full views of humans and to avoid pecking photos with no human, despite great differences in the form, size, and color of both the humans displayed and in the non-human pictures. In follow-up studies, pigeons categorized other natural objects (e.g. trees) and after training they were able without reward to sort photos they had not seen before .[36][37] Similar work has been done with natural auditory categories, for example, bird songs [38]
Functional or associative categories[]
Perceptually unrelated stimuli may come to be responded to as members of a class if they have a common use or lead to common consequences. An oft-cited study by Vaughan (1988) provides an example.[39] Vaughan divided a large set of unrelated pictures into two arbitrary sets, A and B. Pigeons got food for pecking at pictures in set A but not for pecks at pictures in set B. After they had learned this task fairly well, the outcome was reversed: items in set B led to food and items in set A did not. Then the outcome was reversed again, and then again, and so on. Vaughan found that after 20 or more reversals, associating reward with a few pictures in one set caused the birds to respond to the other pictures in that set without further reward, as if they were thinking "if these pictures in set A bring food, the others in set A must also bring food." That is, the birds now categorized the pictures in each set as functionally equivalent. Several other procedures have yielded similar results.[1][33]
Relational or abstract categories[]
When tested in a simple stimulus matching-to-sample task (described above) many animals readily learn specific item combinations, such as "touch red if the sample is red, touch green if the sample is green." But this does not demonstrate that they distinguish between "same" and "different" as general concepts. Better evidence is provided if, after training, an animal successfully makes a choice that matches a novel sample that it has never seen before. Monkeys and chimpanzees do learn to do this, as do pigeons if they are given a great deal of practice with many different stimuli. However, because the sample is presented first, successful matching might mean that the animal is simply choosing the most recently seen "familiar" item rather than the conceptually "same" item. A number of studies have attempted to distinguish these possibilities, with mixed results.[1][33]
Rule learning[]
The use of rules has sometimes been considered an ability restricted to humans, but a number of experiments have shown evidence of simple rule learning in primates[40] and also in other animals. Much of the evidence has come from studies of sequence learning in which the "rule" consists of the order in which a series of events occurs. Rule use is shown if the animal learns to discriminate different orders of events and transfers this discrimination to new events arranged in the same order. For example, Murphy et al. (2008)[41] trained rats to discriminate between visual sequences. For one group ABA and BAB were rewarded, where A="bright light" and B="dim light." Other stimulus triplets were not rewarded. The rats learned the visual sequence, although both bright and dim lights were equally associated with reward. More importantly, in a second experiment with auditory stimuli, rats responded correctly to sequences of novel stimuli that were arranged in the same order as those previously learned. Similar sequence learning has been demonstrated in birds and other animals as well.[42]
Memory[]
The categories that have been developed to analyze human memory (short term memory, long term memory, working memory) have been applied to the study of animal memory, and some of the phenomena characteristic of human short term memory (e.g. the serial position effect) have been detected in animals, particularly monkeys.[43] However most progress has been made in the analysis of spatial memory; some of this work has sought to clarify the physiological basis of spatial memory and the role of the hippocampus; other work has explored the spatial memory of scatter-hoarder animals such as Clark's Nutcracker, certain jays, tits and certain squirrels, whose ecological niches require them to remember the locations of thousands of caches[1][44]., often following radical changes in the environment.
Memory has been widely investigated in foraging honeybees, Apis mellifera, which use both transient short-term working memory that is non-feeder specific and a feeder specific long-term reference memory.[45][46][47] Memory induced in a free-flying honeybee by a single learning trial lasts for days and, by three learning trials, for a lifetime.[48] Slugs, Limax flavus, have a short-term memory of approximately 1 min and long-term memory of 1 month.[49]
Spatial memory[]
The ability to properly navigate and search through the environment is a critical task for many animals. Much of this movement seems to be directed, in the sense that the animals in question seem to be purposely moving towards a particular spot for a reason. Purposeful navigation implies some sort of cognitive map of the external environment.[50] Research in this area (Brown & Cook, 2006[51]) has focused on such diffuse topics as landmark and beacon use by ants and bees, the encoding and use of geometric properties of the environment by pigeons, and the ability of rats to represent a spatial pattern in either radial arm mazes or water mazes. Sometimes included under the envelope of spatial cognition is work in humans and other animals in visual search tasks, which aim to experimentally address questions about searching through one's environment for a particular object.[citation needed] Most progress has been made in the analysis of spatial memory, partly in relation to studies of the physiological basis of spatial memory and the role of the hippocampus, and partly in relation to scatter-hoarder animals such as Clark's Nutcracker, certain jays, titmice and certain squirrels, whose ecological niches require them to remember the locations of thousands of caches, often following radical changes in the environment. Within the literature it has been found that certain species of paridae and corvidae (such as the black-capped chickadee and the scrub jay) are able to use spatial memory to remember where, when and what type of food they have cached.[52] Recent studies with rats and squirrels, have also suggested that they are able to use spatial memory to locate previously hidden food.[52] Experiments using the radial maze have allowed researchers to control for a number of variables, such as the type of food hidden, the locations where the food is hidden, the retention interval, as well as any odour cues that could skew results of memory research.[52] In particular, studies have indicated that rats have memory for where they have hidden food and what type of food they have hidden.[52] This is shown in retrieval behaviour, such that the rats are selective in going more often to the arms of the maze where they have previously hidden preferred food than to arms with less preferred food or where no food was hidden.[52]
It has been hypothesized that apes may be quite good at spatial cognition, while dogs have a more difficult time. Apes may be skillful at using spatial cues to find food, as that is an activity typical to their every day lives. The domesticated dog, on the other hand, typically lives with a human who provides necessities such as food and shelter. This hypothesis is based on the process of domestication over the years starting about 15,000 years ago.[53] Dogs have developed a reliance on humans and have slowly lost their instictive nature. As a result, domesticated dogs do not face the same basic needs for survival as their closely related cousins the wolves. Who, just like apes, are much better at spatial cognition.[54][55] Thus, the evidence for the spatial memory of some species of animals, such as rats, indicates that they do use spatial memory to locate and retrieve hidden food stores.[52]
Chimpanzee Working Memory- Better than humans?[]
It has recently been claimed that chimpanzee working memory is superior to even that of humans. The task used to measure working memory in both species involved glancing at a set of numbers for varing amounts of time before they were occluded by white boxes, which individuals then had to press in ascending order (see video). [56]. In the masking task when the human/chimpanzee pressed the first number, the other numbers disappeared, whereas in the limited-hold memory task, after touching the start key, the numbers appeared for varying amounts of time depending on the research condition (210-650ms). The chimpanzees used in the tasks could be considered "enculturared" since all of them have had extensive human contact and some had participated in previous psychology experiments in the Primate Research Institute of Kyoto University.
The headline claim that chimpanzees generallyoutperform human research subjects is simply misinformation about the task; a fact not helped by the researchers reporting in other papers that "young chimpanzees have an extraordinary working memory capability... better than that of human adults" [57]. In response time and accuracy, only 1/6 of the chimpanzees outperformed the three humans (Ayumu), and 3/6 of the older chimpanzees performed worse in both respects. The three young chimpanzees (including Ayumu) had response times faster than humans, however the accuracy of 2/3 of the chimpanzees was substantially worse. All three older chimpanzees were outperformed by humans, and only 1 chimpanzee (Ayumu) outperformed the human average in both accuracy and response latency.
Other researchers have argued that humans were not given enough practise on the task, compared to the apes. When trained using similar method used to the chimpanzees, humans outperformed Ayumu, the best performing chimpanzee, on the 210ms delay task. [58] Even though the evidence was scant in the first place, this further dismantles the evidence of "eidetic imagery" or superior working memory in chimpanzees.
Episodic Memory[]
- Main article: Episodic-like memory
Various different definitions of episodic memoryexist, however these definitions and concepts have been developed in human research are difficult to fairly apply to nonhuman animals. Endel Tulving (1973) first argued that declarative memory could be divided into memory of facts vs events (episodic vs. semantic).[59] However, the concept of episodic memory has been expanded to include the notion that episodic memory has a distinct phenomenological experience to semantic memory. Tulving argues that episodic memory of events has a unique "autonoetic" experience from semantic memory; the difference can be best described as "remembering" and "knowing" experiences. In addition, the process of remembering, for Tulving, invovles a form of mental time travel (or planning) which he a priori argues must be missing in nonhuman animals. [60]
Given that we can only speculate about the conscious experience of animals, it makes no sense to base a reasonable comparative research program on Tulving's definitions. Clayton and Dickson (1998) created a new definition based on observable behavior; they argued that so-called episodic-like memory requires an integrated reprsentation of the what, where and when of an event. Using this definition, many researchers have tried to show that nonhuman animals possess "episodic-like" memory.
Social learning in animals[]
- Main article: Social learning in animals
Adaptive behavior in animals is often explained by natural selection on genes, however natural selection on the ability to learn socially may also be an important adaptive strategy, for various reasons. Unlike behaviour driven by genes, social learning allows individuals to learn information at a far more rapid pace. Unlike indicidual learning, social learning can be far less dangerous, as is the case with learning about new predators. [61] Social learning has been quite broadly defined as: " ...any incidence in which individuals acquire new behaviour or information about their environment via observation of, or interaction with, other animals or their products"[62]. Clearly, social learning does not refer to a single ability or mechanism, but a wide range of ways by which information may be derived from the environment. Various types of social learning have been proposed.
Culture in Animals[]
- Main article: Animal culture
Comparative psychologists define culture slightly differnetly to the layman or social scientist. However, by the broader defintion of culture set by animal researchers (a normative, socially learned, non-instinctive and collective behavior; see main article), culture is a taxonomically widespread trait.
Culture does not necessarily rely on more complex cognition, as experiments have shown that fish can acquire cultural differences in antipredator behavior through associative learning of a new prey and the "fright response" of conspecifics. In Zebra Danio Fish, a new predator which attacks a Danio will cause an alarm reaction from the shoal [63]. This leads to individuals who were not attacked associating the new predator with the alarm reaction, leading to cultural differences in predator knowledge, through little more than basic stimulus-assocation responses.
Perception[]
Like humans, non-human animals process information from eyes, ears, and other sensory organs to perceive the environment. Perceptual processes have been studied in many species, with results that are often similar to those in humans. Equally interesting are those perceptual processes that differ from, or go beyond those found in humans, such as echolocation in bats and dolphins, motion detection by skin receptors in fish, and extraordinary visual acuity, motion sensitivity and ability to see ultraviolet light in some birds.[64]
Tool and weapon use[]
- Further information: Category:Tool-using species
- Further information: Tool use by animals
Because tool use is traditionally assumed to be a uniquely human trait, discussion of the cognitive underpinnings of animal tool use very often includes consideration of insight and comparisons of the overall intelligence and brain size. Another animal that uses tools are birds, in particular the New Caledonian crow. One in particular who was given the name “Betty.” She spontaneously made a wire tool to solve a novel problem in the laboratory attracted considerable attention. She was being tested to see whether she would select a wire hook rather than a straight wire to pull a little bucket of meat out of a well. Betty tried poking the straight wire at the meat. After a series of failures with this direct approach, she withdrew the wire and began directing it at the bottom of the well, which was secured to its base with duct tape. The wire soon became stuck, whereupon Betty pulled it sideways, bending it and unsticking it. She then inserted the hook into the well and extracted the meat. In all but one of 10 subsequent trials with only straight wire provided, she also made and used a hook in the same manner, but not before trying the straight wire first.[65][66] Some other species of birds, such as the Woodpecker Finch of the Galapagos Islands, use particular tools as an essential part of their foraging behavior. However, these behaviors are often quite inflexible and cannot be applied effectively in new situations. Several species have now been shown to be capable of more flexible tool use. A well-known example is Jane Goodall's observation of chimpanzees "fishing" for termites in their natural environment, and captive great apes are often observed to use tools effectively; several species of corvids have also been trained to use tools in controlled experiments, or use bread crumbs for bait-fishing.[67][unreliable source?]
Some species, such as the Woodpecker Finch of the Galapagos Islands, use particular tools as an essential part of their foraging behavior. However, these behaviors are often quite inflexible and cannot be applied effectively in new situations. Several species have now been shown to be capable of more flexible tool use. A well known example is Jane Goodall's observation of chimpanzees "fishing" for termites in their natural environment, and captive great apes are often observed to use tools effectively; several species of corvids have also been trained to use tools in controlled experiments, or use bread crumbs for bait-fishing [2].
Research in 2007 shows that chimpanzees in the Fongoli savannah sharpen sticks to use as spears when hunting, considered the first evidence of systematic use of weapons in a species other than humans.[68][69]
Some cephalopods are known to use coconut shells for protection or camouflage.[70]
Reasoning and problem solving[]
Closely related to tool use is the study of reasoning and problem solving. It has been observed that the manner in which chimpanzees solve problems, such as that of retrieving bananas positioned out of reach, is not through trial-and-error. Instead, they were observed to proceed in a manner that was "unwaveringly purposeful."[71]
It is clear that animals of quite a range of species are capable of solving a range of problems that are argued to involve abstract reasoning;[72] modern research has tended to show that the performances of Wolfgang Köhler's chimpanzees, who could achieve spontaneous solutions to problems without training, were by no means unique to that species, and that apparently similar behavior can be found in animals usually thought of as much less intelligent, if appropriate training is given.[citation needed] Causal reasoning has also been observed in rooks and New Caledonian crows.[73][74]
Language[]
- Main article: Animal language
The modeling of human language in animals is known as animal language research. In addition to the ape-language experiments mentioned above, there have also been more or less successful attempts to teach language or language-like behavior to some non-primate species, including parrots and Great Spotted Woodpeckers. Louis Herman published research on artificial language comprehension in the bottlenosed dolphin using cognitive research methods at the height of the skepticism produced by Herbert Terrace's criticism of chimpanzee language experiments through his own results with the animal Nim Chimpsky. In particular, the focus on the comprehension mode only allowed cognitive methods of utilizing blinded observers to grade the animals' gross physical behavior, rather than trying to interpret putative language production. Herman's results (Herman, Richards, & Wolz, 1984) were published in the (human) journal Cognition, regarding work on the dolphins Akeakamai and Phoenix. All such research has been controversial among cognitive linguists.
Consciousness[]
- Main article: Animal consciousness
The sense in which animals can be said to have consciousness or a self-concept has been hotly debated; it is often referred to as the debate over animal minds. The best known research technique in this area is the mirror test devised by Gordon G. Gallup, in which an animal's skin is marked in some way while it is asleep or sedated, and it is then allowed to see its reflection in a mirror; if the animal spontaneously directs grooming behavior towards the mark, that is taken as an indication that it is aware of itself. Self-awareness, by this criterion, has been reported for chimpanzees and also for other great apes, the European Magpie,[75] some cetaceans and a solitary elephant, but not for monkeys. The mirror test has attracted controversy among some researchers because it is entirely focused on vision, the primary sense in humans, while other species rely more heavily on other senses such as the olfactory sense in dogs.[citation needed]
It has been suggested that metacognition in some animals provides some evidence for cognitive self-awareness.[76] The great apes, dolphins, and rhesus monkeys have demonstrated the ability to monitor their own mental states and use an "I don't know" response to avoid answering difficult questions. These species might also be aware of the strength of their memories. Unlike the mirror test, which relies primarily on body images and bodily self-awareness, uncertainty monitoring paradigms are focused on the kinds of mental states that might be linked to mental self-awareness.[citation needed]
A different approach to determine whether a non-human animal is conscious derives from passive speech research with a macaw (see Arielle). Some researchers propose that by passively listening to an animal's voluntary speech, it is possible to learn about the thoughts of another creature and to determine that the speaker is conscious. This type of research was originally used to investigate a child's crib speech by Weir (1962) and in investigations of early speech in children by Greenfield and others (1976). With speech-capable birds, the methods of passive-speech research open a new avenue for investigation.[citation needed]
In July, 2012 during the "Consciousness in Human and Nonhuman Animals" conference in Cambridge a group of scientists announced and signed a declaration with the following conclusions:
Convergent evidence indicates that non-human animals have the neuroanatomical, neurochemical, and neurophysiological substrates of conscious states along with the capacity to exhibit intentional behaviors. Consequently, the weight of evidence indicates that humans are not unique in possessing the neurological substrates that generate consciousness. Non-human animals, including all mammals and birds, and many other creatures, including octopuses, also possess these neurological substrates.[77]
Animal insight[]
- See also: Reason
Along with consciousness comes insight. Do animals have that “outside-the-box” or the “Aha! experience", sometimes called the Eureka effect? That thinking process that helps them solve everyday problems and help them to adapt in the outside world. Some may argue that this is called instinct, but insight is different. Wolfgang Köhler is usually credited with introducing the concept of insight into the psychological world.[66] Köhler worked with apes that became masters of solving puzzles he gave them. Köhler followed Edward Thorndike’s theory that animals solve problems gradually, first finding success through a process of trial and error and slowly becoming more skillful. Köhler came to disagree with this theory saying, “Thorndike’s animals could only escape by chance at first because their structure did not permit other kinds of situations.”[66]
Contemporary studies of human insight address the cognitive and neural mechanisms underlying problem-solving behavior that fit this definition. In the case of animals, this usually means associative learning. Because we cannot simply ask animals about their “aha” experiences we should define insightful behavior in terms of processes such as mental trial and error or casual understanding.[66]
Numeracy[]
Some animals are capable of distinguishing between different amounts and rudimentary counting. Elephants have been known to perform simple arithmetic, and rhesus monkeys and pigeons, in some sense, can count.[78][79][80] Ants are able to use quantitative values and transmit this information.[81][82] For instance, ants of several species are able to estimate quite precisely numbers of encounters with members of other colonies on their feeding territories.[83][84] Numeracy has been described in the yellow mealworm beetle, Tenebrio molitor,[85] and the honeybee.[86]
Western lowland gorillas given the choice between two food trays demonstrated the ability to choose the tray with more food items at a rate higher than chance after training.[87] In a similar task, chimpanzees chose the option with larger amount of food.[88] Salamanders given a choice between two displays with differing amounts of fruit flies, used as a food reward, reliably choose the display with more flies, as shown in a particular experiment.[89]
Other experiments have been conducted that show animals’ abilities to differentiate between non-food quantities. American black bears demonstrated quantity differentiation abilities in a task with a computer screen. The bears were trained to touch a computer monitor with a paw or nose to choose a quantity of dots in one of two boxes on the screen. Each bear was trained with reinforcement to pick a larger or smaller amount. During training, the bears were rewarded with food for a correct response. All bears performed better than what random error predicted on the trials with static, non-moving dots, indicating that they could differentiate between the two quantities. The bears choosing correctly in congruent (number of dots coincided with area of the dots) and incongruent (number of dots did not coincide with area of the dots) trials suggests that they were indeed choosing between quantities that appeared on the screen, not just a larger or smaller retinal image, which would indicate they are only judging size.[90]
Bottlenose dolphins have shown the ability to choose an array with fewer dots compared to one with more dots. Experimenters set up two boards showing various numbers of dots in a poolside setup. The dolphins were initially trained to choose the board with the fewer number of dots. This was done by rewarding the dolphin when it chose the board with the fewer number of dots. In the experimental trials, two boards were set up, and the dolphin would emerge from the water and point to one board. The dolphins chose the arrays with fewer dots at a rate much larger than chance, indicating they can differentiate between quantities.[91] A particular grey parrot, after training, has shown the ability to differentiate between the numbers zero through six using vocalizations. After number and vocalization training, this was done by asking the parrot how many objects there were in a display. The parrot was able to identify the correct amount at a rate higher than chance.[92] Angelfish, when put in an unfamiliar environment will group together with conspecifics, an action named shoaling. Given the choice between two groups of differing size, the angelfish will choose the larger of the two groups. This can be seen with a discrimination ratio of 2:1 or greater, such that, as long as one group has at least twice the fish as another group, it will join the larger one.[93]
Monitor lizards have been shown to be capable of numeracy, and some species can distinguish among numbers up to six.[94]
Emotion[]
- Main article: Emotion in animals
The question of whether animals have emotions, that is, feelings, and if those are qualitatively the same as humans' feelings or in fact simple stimulus responses, is under debate at the moment.
Timing[]
- Further information: Time perception
Time of day: Circadian rhythms[]
- Main article: Circadian rhythms
The behavior of most animals is synchronized with the earth's daily light-dark cycle. Thus, many animals are active during the day, others are active at night, still others near dawn and dusk. Though one might think that these "circadian rhythms" are controlled simply by the presence or absence of light, nearly every animal that has been studied has been shown to have a "biological clock" that yields cycles of activity even when the animal is in constant illumination or darkness.[1] Circadian rhythms are so automatic and fundamental to living things — they occur even in plants[95] - that they are usually discussed separately from cognitive processes, and the reader is referred to the main article (Circadian rhythms) for further information.
Interval timing[]
Survival often depends on an animal's ability to time intervals. For example, rufous hummingbirds feed on the nectar of flowers, and they often return to the same flower, but only after the flower had had enough time to replenish its supply of nectar. In one experiment hummingbirds fed on artificial flowers that quickly emptied of nectar but were refilled at some fixed time (e.g. twenty minutes) later. The birds learned to come back to the flowers at about the right time, learning the refill rates of up to eight separate flowers and remembering how long ago they had visited each one.[96]
The details of interval timing have been studied in a number of species. One of the most common methods is the "peak procedure". In a typical experiment, a rat in an operant chamber presses a lever for food. A light comes on, a lever-press brings a food pellet at a fixed later time, say 10 seconds, and then the light goes off. Timing is measured during occasional test trials on which no food is presented and the light stays on. On these test trials the rat presses the lever more and more until about 10 sec and then, when no food comes, gradually stops pressing. The time at which the rat presses most on these test trials is taken to be its estimate of the payoff time.
Experiments using the peak procedure and other methods have shown that animals can time short intervals quite exactly, can time more than one event at once, and can integrate time with spatial and other cues. Such tests have also been used for quantitative tests of theories of animal timing, though no one theory has yet gained unanimous agreement.[1]
Biological constraints[]
Due to differences in instinctive behaviour, some studies on cognition can be misleading when investigating a species' capacity for learning. For example, animals can be trained to escape an electric shock from the floor by moving to another part of the experimental chamber when hearing a tone preceding the shock. Domestic dogs would easily learn this task and jump away from the electrified floor, however, a hedgehog would not learn the task because its instinct when threatened is to curl up into a ball thereby remaining on the floor and not learning to avoid the shock.
Instinctive drift is another biological constraint which can influence interpretation of animal cognition studies. Instinctive drift is the tendency of an animal to revert to instinctive behaviors that can interfere with learned responses. The concept originated with Keller and Marian Breland when they taught a raccoon to put coins into a box. The raccoon instinctively drifted to its instinctive behavior of rubbing the coins with its paws, as it would do when washing food.[97]
Cognitive faculty by species[]
A common image is the scala naturae, the ladder of nature on which animals of different species occupy successively higher rungs, with humans typically at the top.[98]
A more fruitful approach has been to recognize that different animals may have different kinds of cognitive processes, which are better understood in terms of the ways in which they are cognitively adapted to their different ecological niches, than by positing any kind of hierarchy. (See Shettleworth (1998), Reznikova (2007).)
One question that can be asked coherently is how far different species are intelligent in the same ways as humans are, i.e., are their cognitive processes similar to ours. Not surprisingly, our closest biological relatives, the great apes, tend to do best on such an assessment. Among the birds, corvids and parrots have typically been found to perform well. Octopodes have also been shown to exhibit a number of higher-level skills such as tool use,[99] but the amount of research on cephalopod intelligence is still limited.[citation needed]
Baboons have been shown to be capable of recognizing words.[100][101][102]
Continuing controversy[]
The broad program of research into animal cognition has achieved a good deal. Nonetheless, its results and philosophy continue to be debated, on a number of grounds:
- Particular issues within animal cognition, particularly the interpretation of language-learning and self-awareness experiments, have generated major controversies both about the extent of the animals' achievements, and about the correct interpretation of the behavior observed.
- Cognitive scientists have been interested in comparing and contrasting human cognition with artificial intelligence or machine cognition, but have been less interested in including animal cognition in the analysis - despite the fact that the common biological origins of human and animal cognition suggest that there might be greater resemblance, at least in some respects, between human and animal cognition than between human and machine cognition. There is also a minority of cognitive scientists who simply neglect accumulated psychological knowledge about cognition, whether animal or human.
- Those psychologists who are committed to radical behaviorism and the experimental analysis of behaviour discount cognitive analyses of animal behavior. This is not surprising since for the most part they also reject cognitive analyses of human behaviour. It is perhaps a category error to oppose behavioural and cognitive analyses: insofar as the study of animal cognition exposes new behavioural phenomena, it simply provides more that a radical behaviourist must explain without using mentalistic language.
Relative intelligence of different animal species[]
Some animals, including great apes, crows, dolphins, dogs, elephants, cats, and parrots are typically thought by humans as intelligent in ways that other animals are not. For example, crows are attributed with human-like intelligence in the folklore of many cultures. A number of recent survey studies have demonstrated the consistency of these rankings between people in a given culture and indeed to a considerable extent across cultures (e.g. Nakajima et al., 2002). A common image is the scala naturae, the ladder of nature on which animals of different species occupy successively higher rungs, with humans typically at the top. Comparative psychologists have sought in vain for ways of providing an objective underpinning for these essentially subjective and anthropocentric judgements. Part of the difficulty is the lack of agreement about what we mean by intelligence even in humans (it obviously makes a big difference whether language is considered as essential for intelligence, for example). As a result, most scientists studying animal cognition regard questions about which animals are the most intelligent as vacuous. A more fruitful approach has been to recognise that different animals may have different kinds of cognitive processes, which are better understood in terms of the ways in which they are cognitively adapted to their different ecological niches, than by positing any kind of hierarchy. This is the approach taken by the most comprehensive reference text of animal cognition, Shettleworth (1998).
One question that can be asked coherently is how far different species are intelligent in the same ways as humans are, i.e. are their cognitive processes similar to ours. Not surprisingly, our closest biological relatives, the great apes, tend to do best on such an assessment. It is less clear that other species traditionally held to be intelligent do unusually well against this standard, though among the birds, corvids and parrots have typically been found to perform well. Domesticated animals often perform well in tests of human-like abilities, but this may simply reflect their better adaptation to the human world and the proximity of humans.
Despite ambitious claims, evidence of unusually high human-like intelligence among cetaceans is patchy, partly because the cost and difficulty of carrying out research with marine mammals mean that experiments frequently suffer from small sample sizes and inadequate controls and replication. Octopuses have also been claimed to exhibit a number of higher-level problem-solving skills, but the amount of research on cephalopod intelligence is too limited for it to be conclusive.
See also[]
- Anthropomorphism
- Bird intelligence
- Cat intelligence
- Cephalopod intelligence
- Cetacean intelligence
- Cognitive ethology
- Comparative Cognition
- Deception in animals
- Dog intelligence
- Elephant intelligence
- Hominid intelligence
- Infant intelligence
- List of animals by number of neurons
- Pain in invertebrates#Cognitive abilities
- Performing animals
- Problem of other minds
- Task-performing animals
- Vocal learning
References[]
- Goodall, J. (1991). Through a window. London: Penguin.
- Griffin, D. R. (1992). Animal minds. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
- Kamil A. C., Balda R. P. (1990). Differential memory for different cache sites by Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 162-168.
- Hilgard, E. R. (1958). Theories of learning, 2nd edn. London: Methuen.
- Nakajima, S., Arimitsu, K., & Lattal, K. M. (2002). Estimation of animal intelligence by university students in Japan and the United States. Anthrozoös, 15, 194-205.
- Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts.
- Romanes, G. J. (1886). Animal intelligence, 4th edn. London: Kegan Paul, Trench.
- Shettleworth, S. J. (1998). Cognition, evolution and behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.
- Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: a theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
- ↑ 1.0 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 1.5 1.6 1.7 1.8 1.9 Cite error: Invalid
<ref>
tag; no text was provided for refs namedShettleworth
- ↑ Descartes, R. (1649), Passions of the Soul
- ↑ Darwin, C. 1871, The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex
- ↑ Romanes, J. G. 1883, Animal Intelligence
- ↑ Thorndike, E. L. 1911, Animal intelligence.
- ↑ Pavlov, I.P. 1928, Lectures on conditioned reflexes
- ↑ Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the Behaviorist Views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158-177
- ↑ Miller, S. & Konorski, J. (1928) Sur une forme particulière des reflexes conditionels. Comptes Rendus des Seances de la Societe de Biologie et de ses Filiales, 99, 1155-1157
- ↑ Skinner, B. F. (1932) The Behavior of Organisms
- ↑ Hull, C. L. (1943) The Principles of Behavior
- ↑ Skinner, B. F. About Behaviorism 1976
- ↑ Köhler, W. (1917) The Mentality of Apes
- ↑ Tolman, E. C. (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and men Psychological Review, 55, 189-208
- ↑ Niesser, U. (1967) ‘’Cognitive Psychology’’
- ↑ p. 3, Hebb, D. O. 1958 ‘’ A Textbook of Psychology’’
- ↑ p. 2 , Menzel, R. & Fischer, J. (2010) ‘’Animal Thinking: Contemporary Issues in Comparative Cognition’’
- ↑ 17.0 17.1 17.2 17.3 17.4 Wasserman & Zentall (eds) (2006) ‘’Comparative Cognition’’
- ↑ Grandin, Temple (2009) Animals Make Us Human: Creating the Best life for Animals (with Catherine Johnson)
- ↑ Smith, E. E., and Kosslyn, S. M. (2007) "Cognitive Psychology: Mind and Brain" Pearson Prentice Hall.
- ↑ Blough, D. S. (2006) Reaction-time explorations of visual attention, perception, and decision in pigeons. In E. A. Wasserman & T. R. Zentall (Eds) Comparative Cognition: Experimental Explorations of Animal Intelligence’’ pp. 89-105. New York: Oxford.
- ↑ N. J. Mackintosh (1983) Conditioning and Associative Learning’’ New York: Oxford
- ↑ Kamin, L. J. (1969) Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning. In Campbell and Church (eds.) ‘’Punishment and aversive behavior’’, New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts pp. 279-296
- ↑ Mackintosh, N. J. (1994) ‘’Animal Learning and Cognition’’ San Diego: Academic Press
- ↑ Zentall, T. R. (2004) Selective and divided attention in animals. ‘’Behavioural Processes’’ 69, 1-16
- ↑ Blough, D. S. (1969) Attention shifts in a maintained discrimination. ‘’Science’’,166, 125-126
- ↑ Tinbergen, L.(1960) The natural control of insects in pine woods: I. Factors influencing the intensity of predation by songbirds. ‘’Archives Néerlandasises de Zoologie’’ 13, 265-343.
- ↑ Pietrewicz, A. T. & Kamil, A. C. (1977) Visual detection of crypic prey by blue jays ‘’(Cyanocitta cristata). Science,’’ 195,580-582.
- ↑ Pietrewicz, A. T. Kamil, A. C. (1979) Search image formation in the blue jay ‘’(Cyanocitta cristata). Science,’’ 204, 1332-1333)
- ↑ Blough, P. M. (1989). Attentional priming and visual search in pigeons. ‘’Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,’’ 17, 292-298.
- ↑ Kamil, A.C. & Bond, A. B. (2006) Selective attention, priming, and foraging behavior. In E. A. Wasserman and T. R. Zentall(eds) ‘’Comparative Cognition: Experimental Exploration of Animal Intelligence’’ New York: Oxford
- ↑ Blough, D. S. & Blough, P. M. (1990) Reaction-time assessments of visual processes in pigeons. In M. Berkley & W. Stebbins (Eds.) ‘’Comparative perception (pp. 245-276). New York:Wiley.
- ↑ E. E. Smith & D. L. Medin (1981) ‘’Categories and Concepts’’ Harvard Univ. Press
- ↑ 33.0 33.1 33.2 33.3 33.4 Zentall, T. R., Wasserman, E. A., Lazareva, O. F., Thompson, R. R. K., Ratterman, M. J. (2008). Concept Learning in Animals. ‘’Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews’’, 3 , 13-45. Retrieved from http://psyc.queensu.ca/ccbr/index.html DOI:10.3819/ccbr.2008.30002
- ↑ Dooling, R. J., & Okanoya, K. (1995). Psychophysical methods for assessing perceptual categories. In G. M.Klump, R. J.Dooling, R. R.Fay, & W. C.Stebbins (Eds.),’’ Methods in Comparative Psychoacoustics’’ (pp. 307–318). Basel, Switzerland: Birkhäuser Verlag.
- ↑ R. J. Herrnstein (1964) ‘’Complex Visual Concept in the Pigeon’’ Science, 146, 549-551
- ↑ R. J. Herrnstein (1979) ‘’Acquisition, Generalization, and Discrimination Reversal of a Natural Concept’’ J. of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 5, 116-129
- ↑ R. S. Bhatt, E. A. Wasserman, W.F.J. Reynolds, & K. S.. Knauss (1988) ‘’Conceptual behavior in pigeons: Categorization of both familiar and novel examples from four classes of natural and articifial stimuli.’’ J. of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 219-234
- ↑ H-W Tu, E. Smith & R. J. Dooling, (2011). Acoustic and perceptual categories of vocal elements in the warble song of budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulates) ‘’J. of Comparative Psychology, 125, 420-430)’’
- ↑ W. Vaughan, Jr. (1988) Formation of equivalence sets in pigeons. ‘’Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Process 14, 36-42
- ↑ See, e.g., D’Amato, M., & M. Columbo (1988). Representation of serial order in monkeys (‘’Cebus apella’’). ‘’Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,’’ 14, 11-139
- ↑ Murphy, R. A., E. Mondragon & V. A. Murphy (2008) Rule learning by rats. ‘’Science’’, 319, 1849-1851.
- ↑ Kundrey, S. M. A., B Strandell, H. Mathis & J. D. Rowan (2010) Learning of monotonic and nonmonotonic sequences in domesticated horses (‘’Equus callabus’’) and chickens (‘’Gallus domesticus’’). ‘’Learning and Motivation,’’ 14, 213-223.
- ↑ (1985). Memory processing of serial lists by pigeons, monkeys, and people. Science 229 (4710): 287–289.
- ↑ (1992). Long-term spatial memory in Clark's nutcracker, Nucifraga columbiana. Animal Behaviour 44 (4): 761–769.
- ↑ (1993). Memory dynamics and foraging strategies of honeybees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 32: 17–29.
- ↑ (1993). Associative learning in honey-bees. Apidologie 24 (3): 157–168.
- ↑ Wustenberg, D., Gerber, B. and Menzel, R. (1998). Long- but not medium-term retention of olfactory memory in honeybees is impaired by actinomycin D and anisomycin. European Journal of Neuroscience, 10': 2742-2745
- ↑ (1995). Learning and memory in the honeybee. Journal of Neuroscience 15 (3 Pt 1): 1617–1630.
- ↑ (1992). Behavioral analysis of internal memory states using cooling-induced retrograde anmesia in Limax flavus. The Journal of Neuroscience 12 (3): 729–735.
- ↑ Lund, Nick (2002). Animal cognition, Psychology Press.
- ↑ Animal Spatial Cognition:Comparative, Neural & Computational Approaches
- ↑ 52.0 52.1 52.2 52.3 52.4 52.5 (2003). Spatial memory for food hidden by rats (Rattus norvegicus) on the radial maze: studies of memory for where, what, and when. Journal of Comparative Psychology 117 (2): 176–187.
- ↑ Savolainen, Peter, Ya-ping Zhang, Jing Luo, Joakim Lundeberg, Thomas Leitner (22). Genetic Evidence for an East Asian Origin of Domestic Dogs. Science 298 (5598): 1060–1062.
- ↑ Fiset, Sylvain, Vickie Plourde (29). Object Permanence in Domestic Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) and Gray Wolves (Canis lupus). Journal of Comparative Psychology: 2.
- ↑ Brauer, Juliane, Juliane Kaminski, Julia Riedel, Josep Call, Michael Tomasello (2006). Making Inferences About the Location of Hidden Food: Social Dog, Causal Ape. Journal of Comparative Psychology 120 (1): 38–47.
- ↑ Inoue, S. & Matsuzawa, T. (2007) Working memory of numerals in chimpanzees. Current Biology, 17, 23.
- ↑ Matsuzawa, T. (2009) Symbolic representation of number in chimpanzees. 'Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19, p.97
- ↑ Cook, P. & Wilson, M. (2010) Do young chimpanzees have extraordinary working memory? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 5, 599-600.
- ↑ cited in; Tulving, E. (2002) Episodic Memory: From Mind to Brain.Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 1-25.
- ↑ Tulving, E. (2002) Episodic Memory: From Mind to Brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 1-25.
- ↑ Brown, C. & Laland, K.N. (2003) Social learning in fishes: a review. Fish and Fisheries, 4, 280-288
- ↑ Brown, C. & Laland, K.N. (2003) Social learning in fishes: a review. Fish and Fisheries, 4, 280-288
- ↑ Suboksi, M.D. et al. (1990) Alarm Reaction in Acquisition and Social Transmission of Simulated-Predator Recogntion by Zebra Danio Fish (rachydanio rerio). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 1, 101-112.
- ↑ Stebbins, W. C. & M. A. Berkley (1990) Comparative Perception,Vol. I, Basic Mechanisms; Vol. II, Complex Signals New York: Wiley.
- ↑ Hunt, G.R (1996). Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature 379 (6562): 249–251.
- ↑ 66.0 66.1 66.2 66.3 Shettleworth, Sara J. (2012). Do Animals Have Insight, and What Is Insight Anyway?. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 66 (4): 217–226.
- ↑ [1][unreliable source?]
- ↑ http://desmoinesregister.com/apps/pbcs.dll/article?AID=/20070223/NEWS/702230385/-1/NEWS04
- ↑ http://news.nationalgeographic.com/news/2007/02/070222-chimps-spears.html
- ↑ Finn, J. K., Tregenza, T., Tregenza, N. (2009). Defensive tool use in a coconut-carrying octopus. Current Biology 19 (23): R1069–R1070.
- ↑ Wolfgang Köhler The Mentality of Apes (1917)
- ↑ For Chimpanzees, see for example David Premack (1983) The Mind of an Ape
- ↑ (April 2007). Non-tool-using rooks, Corvus frugilegus, solve the trap-tube problem. Anim Cogn 10 (2): 225–31.
- ↑ (January 2009). Do New Caledonian crows solve physical problems through causal reasoning?. Proc. R. Soc. B 276 (1655): 247–254.
- ↑ Prior, Schwarz, and Güntürkün, Helmut, Ariane, and Onur (2008). Mirror-Induced Behavior in the Magpie (Pica pica): Evidence of Self-Recognition. PLoS Biology 6 (8): e202.
- ↑ Couchman, Justin J., Coutinho, M. V. C., Beran, M. J., & Smith, J. D. (2010). Beyond Stimulus Cues and Reinforcement Signals: A New Approach to Animal Metacognition. Journal of Comparative Psychology 124 (4): , 356 –368.
- ↑ The Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness. URL accessed on 12 August 2012.
- ↑ Elephants show flair for arithmetic
- ↑ Representation of the Numerosities 1-9 by Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatto
- ↑ Pigeons on Par with Primates in Numerical Competence
- ↑ Zhanna Reznikova, Boris Ryabko, "A Study of Ants' Numerical Competence". Electronic Transactions on Artificial Intelligence, Issue: Vol. 5(2001): Section B: pp. 111-126
- ↑ Reznikova, Zh. I. (2007). Animal Intelligence: From Individual to Social Cognition. Cambridge University Press
- ↑ Reznikova, Zh. I. (1999). Ethological mechanisms of population dynamic in species ant communities. Russian Journal of Ecology, 30, 3, 187–197
- ↑ (2000). How resources and encounters affect the distribution of foraging activity in a seed-harvesting ants. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 47 (3): 195–203.
- ↑ (2009). Quantity discrimination in Tenebrio molitor: evidence of numerosity discrimination in an invertebrate?. Animal Cognition 12 (3): 463–470.
- ↑ (2008). Evidence for counting in insects. Animal Cognition 11 (4): 683–689.
- ↑ Anderson, U.S., Stoinski, T.S., Bloomsmith, M.A., Marr, M.J., Smith, A.D., & Maple, T.L. (2005). Relative numerousness judgment and summation in young and old western lowland gorillas. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 285–295.
- ↑ Boysen S.T., Berntson G.G., Mukobi K.L. (2001) Size matters: impact of item size and quantity on array choice by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) J. Comp. Psychol, 115, 106–110.
- ↑ Uller C., Jaeger R., Guidry G., Martin C. (2003) Salamanders (Plethodon cinereus) go for more: rudiments of number in an amphibian. Anim Cogn, 6, 105-112.
- ↑ Vonk J., Beran M.J. (2012) Bears ‘count’ too: quantity estimation and comparison in black bears, Ursus americanus, Animal Behaviour, 84, 1, 231-238.
- ↑ Jaakkola K., Fellner W., Erb L., Rodriguez M., Guarino E. (2005) Understanding of the concept of numerically "less" by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 286–303.
- ↑ Pepperberg I. (2006) Grey parrot numerical competence: a review. Anim Cogn, 9, 377–391.
- ↑ Gómez-Laplaza, L.M. & Gerlai, R. (2010). Can angelfish (Pterophyllum scalare) count? Discrimination between different shoal sizes follows Weber’s law. Anim. Cogn, 14, 1-9.
- ↑ King, Dennis & Green, Brian. 1999. Goannas: The Biology of Varanid Lizards. University of New South Wales Press. ISBN 0-86840-456-X, p. 43.
- ↑ (2003). The physiology of circadian rhythms in plants. New Phytologist 160 (2): 281–303.
- ↑ (2006). Timing in free-living rufous hummingbirds, Selasphorus rufus. Current Biology 16 (5): 512–515.
- ↑ Breland, K. and Breland, M. (1961). The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist, 16: 681-684
- ↑ Campbell, C.B.G., & Hodos, W. (1991). The Scala Naturae revisited: Evolutionary scales and anagenesis in comparative psychology. J. Comp. Psychol. 105:211-221
- ↑ (2009). Defensive tool use in a coconut-carrying octopus. Current Biology 19 (23): R1069–70.
- ↑ Baboons can learn to recognize words; Monkeys' ability suggests that reading taps into general systems of pattern recognition 12 April 2012 Nature
- ↑ Baboons can recognize written words, study finds; The monkeys don't assign meaning to them, but learn what letter combinations are common to real words, the study authors say April 12, 2012 Los Angeles Times
- ↑ Baboons show their word skills; Reading may stem from a visual aptitude shared by all primates May 5, 2012
External links[]
Animal communication | ||
---|---|---|
Concepts |
Animal training · Animal language · Animal cognition · Bioacoustics · Ethology · Evolutionary linguistics · FOXP2 · Origin of language · Proto-language |
|
Animal-specifics | ||
Notable individual animals |
List of communicative birds · List of communicative apes · Kosik the elephant |
Instructions_for_archiving_academic_and_professional_materials
Animal cognition: Academic support materials
- Animal cognition: Lecture slides
- Animal cognition: Lecture notes
- Animal cognition: Lecture handouts
- Animal cognition: Multimedia materials
- Animal cognition: Other academic support materials
- Animal cognition: Anonymous fictional case studies for training
This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia (view authors). |