Psychology Wiki
Advertisement

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Cognitive Psychology: Attention · Decision making · Learning · Judgement · Memory · Motivation · Perception · Reasoning · Thinking  - Cognitive processes Cognition - Outline Index


Conditioned suppression is a procedure that leads to a decrease in operant behavior while the conditioned aversive is present. It is measured by comparing the rates of behavior with and without the presence of the conditioned aversive (i.e., using a suppression ratio).

A neutral stimulus is presented in appropriate temporal-spatial proximity to an unconditioned aversive stimulus (e.g., shock), during which the neutral stimulus becomes conditioned stimulus. Following this Pavlovian conditioning, ongoing operant behaviors decrease when the conditioned stimulus is presented.

It has been used in a number of situations and is especially useful for studying the acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear.

See also[]

References[]

  • Aaron, M., & Thorne, B. M. (1975). Omission training and extinction in rats with septal damage: Physiology & Behavior Vol 15(2) Aug 1975, 149-154.
  • Abel, E. L. (1982). In utero alcohol exposure and developmental delay of response inhibition: Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research Vol 6(3) Sum 1982, 369-376.
  • Abramowitz, J. S., Tolin, D. F., & Street, G. P. (2001). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression: A meta-analysis of controlled studies: Clinical Psychology Review Vol 21(5) Jul 2001, 683-703.
  • Abramson, C. I., & Feinman, R. D. (1987). Operant punishment of eye elevation in the green crab, Carcinus maenas: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 48(2) Sep 1987, 259-277.
  • Acerbo, M. J., Godoy, A. M., & Delius, J. D. (2003). Haloperidol blocks the acquisition but not the retrieval of a conditioned sensitization to apomorphine: Behavioural Pharmacology Vol 14(8) Dec 2003, 631-640.
  • Acri, J. B., Morse, D. E., Popke, E. J., & Grunberg, N. E. (1994). Nicotine increases sensory gating measured as inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex in rats: Psychopharmacology Vol 114(2) Mar 1994, 369-374.
  • Ader, R., & Cohen, N. (1975). Behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression: Psychosomatic Medicine Vol 37(4) Jul-Aug 1975, 333-340.
  • Ader, R., Grota, L. J., & Cohen, N. (1987). Conditioning phenomena and immune function. New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences.
  • Aguado, L., Symonds, M., & Hall, G. (1994). Interval between preexposure and test determines the magnitude of latent inhibition: Implications for an interference account: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 22(2) May 1994, 188-194.
  • Aitken, M. R. F., Bennett, C. H., McLaren, I. P. L., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1996). Perceptual differentiation during categorization learning by pigeons: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 22(1) Jan 1996, 43-50.
  • Akasaki, T., Sato, H., Yoshimura, Y., Ozeki, H., & Shimegi, S. (2002). Suppressive effects of receptive field surround on neuronal activity in the cat primary visual cortex: Neuroscience Research Vol 43(3) Jul 2002, 207-220.
  • Akase, E., & Mitani, K. (1979). Catecholaminergic drug and conditioned emotional response in the white rat: Annual of Animal Psychology Vol 29(2) Mar 1979, 95-100.
  • Albert, M., & Ayres, J. J. (1989). With number of preexposures constant latent inhibition increases with preexposure CS duration or total CS exposure: Learning and Motivation Vol 20(3) Aug 1989, 278-294.
  • Albert, M., & Ayres, J. J. B. (1997). One-trial simultaneous and backward excitatory fear conditioning in rats: Lick suppression, freezing, and rearing to CS compounds and their elements: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 25(2) May 1997, 210-220.
  • Albert, M., Ricker, S., Bevins, R. A., & Ayres, J. J. (1993). Extending continuous versus discontinuous conditioned stimuli before versus after unconditioned stimuli: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 19(3) Jul 1993, 255-264.
  • Alessandro, D., Dollinger, J., Gordon, J. D., Mariscal, S. K., & et al. (1989). The ontogeny of the pecking response of herring gull chicks: Animal Behaviour Vol 37(3) Mar 1989, 372-382.
  • Algarabel, S. (1983). Origins of the study of conditioned inhibition: I. Pavlov: Psicologica Vol 4(3) 1983, 211-223.
  • Algarabel, S. (1984). Origins of the study of conditioned inhibition: II. Konorski: Psicologica Vol 5(1) 1984, 5-16.
  • Allison, J., Buxton, A., & Moore, K. E. (1987). Bliss points, stop lines, and performance under schedule constraint: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 13(4) Oct 1987, 331-340.
  • Allison, J., & Mack, R. (1982). Polydipsia and autoshaping: Drinking and leverpressing as substitutes for eating: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 10(4) Nov 1982, 465-475.
  • Alonso Martinez, G., & Aguado Aguilar, L. (1988). Blocking among different reinforcers as a test of the motivational nature of Pavlovian conditioned inhibition: Psicologica Vol 9(2) 1988, 97-111.
  • Amorapanth, P., Nader, K., & LeDoux, J. E. (1999). Lesions of periaqueductal gray dissociate-conditioned freezing from conditioned suppression behavior in rats: Learning & Memory Vol 6(5) Sep-Oct 1999, 491-499.
  • Amundson, J. C., Wheeler, D. S., & Miller, R. R. (2005). Enhancement of Pavlovian conditioned inhibition achieved by posttraining inflation of the training excitor: Learning and Motivation Vol 36(3) Aug 2005, 331-352.
  • Anderson, D. C., Crowell, C. R., & Ramirez, R. (1987). Stepwise reductions in US density and intensity: Maintenance and extinction of conditioned suppression: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 15(3) Aug 1987, 312-320.
  • Anderson, L. T., & Ressler, R. H. (1973). Response to a conditioned aversive event in mice as a function of frequency of premating maternal shock: Developmental Psychobiology Vol 6(2) Mar 1973, 113-121.
  • Angermeier, W. F., & Dassler, K. (1992). Inhibitory learning and memory in the lesser octopus (Eledone cirrhosa): Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 30(4) Jul 1992, 309-310.
  • Anisman, H. (1975). Dissociation of disinhibitory effects of scopolamine: Strain and task factors: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 3(4) Jul-Aug 1975, 613-618.
  • Appel, J. B., & Peterson, N. J. (1965). Punishment: Effects of shock intensity on response suppression: Psychological Reports 16(3, Pt 1) 1965, 721-730.
  • Arcediano, F., Escobar, M., & Miller, R. R. (2004). Is stimulus competition an acquisition deficit or a performance deficit? : Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 11(6) Dec 2004, 1105-1110.
  • Arcediano, F., Matute, H., & Miller, R. R. (1997). Blocking of Pavlovian conditioning in humans: Learning and Motivation Vol 28(2) May 1997, 188-199.
  • Arcediano, F., Ortega, N., & Matute, H. (1996). A behavioural preparation for the study of human Pavlovian conditioning: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 49B(3) Aug 1996, 270-283.
  • Archer, E. J. (1954). Postrest performance in motor learning as a function of prerest degree of distribution of practice: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 47(1) Jan 1954, 47-51.
  • Asratian, E. A. (1965). Some characteristics of the formation, functioning, and inhibition of CRs with bilateral connections: Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel'nosti 15(5) 1965, 796-807.
  • Atkinson, R. L. (1991). Inhibition and excitation in fixed-ratio schedules of reinforcement: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Atnip, G., & Hothersall, D. (1975). Response suppression in normal and septal rats: Physiology & Behavior Vol 15(4) Oct 1975, 417-421.
  • Avis, H. H., & Pert, A. (1974). A comparison of the effects of muscarinic and nicotinic anticholinergic drugs on habituation and fear conditioning in rats: Psychopharmacologia Vol 34(3) 1974, 209-222.
  • Ayres, J. J. (1966). Conditioned suppression and the information hypothesis: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 62(1) Aug 1966, 21-25.
  • Ayres, J. J., & Benedict, J. O. (1973). US-alone presentations as an extinction procedure: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 1(1) Feb 1973, 5-8.
  • Ayres, J. J., Benedict, J. O., & Witcher, E. S. (1975). Systematic manipulation of individual events in a truly random control in rats: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 88(1) Jan 1975, 97-103.
  • Ayres, J. J., Bombace, J. C., Shurtleff, D., & Vigorito, M. (1985). Conditioned suppression tests of the context-blocking hypothesis: Testing in the absence of the preconditioned context: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 11(1) Jan 1985, 1-14.
  • Ayres, J. J., & et al. (1979). Some orderly nonmonotonicities in the trial-by-trial acquisition of conditioned suppression: Inhibition with reinforcement? : Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 7(2) May 1979, 174-180.
  • Ayres, J. J., & et al. (1985). Concurrent observations of barpress suppression and freezing: Effects of CS modality and on-line vs. off-line training upon posttrial behavior: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 13(1) Feb 1985, 44-50.
  • Ayres, J. J., Haddad, C., & Albert, M. (1987). One-trial excitatory backward conditioning as assessed by conditioned suppression of licking in rats: Concurrent observations of lick suppression and defensive behaviors: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 15(2) May 1987, 212-217.
  • Ayres, J. J., Mahoney, W. J., Proulx, D. T., & Benedict, J. O. (1976). Backward conditioning as an extinction procedure: Learning and Motivation Vol 7(3) Aug 1976, 368-381.
  • Ayres, J. J., Moore, J. W., & Vigorito, M. (1984). Hall and Pearce negative transfer: Assessments in conditioned suppression and nictitating membrane conditioning experiments: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 12(4) Nov 1984, 428-438.
  • Ayres, J. J., Philbin, D., Cassidy, S., Bellino, L., & et al. (1992). Some parameters of latent inhibition: Learning and Motivation Vol 23(3) Aug 1992, 269-287.
  • Ayres, J. J., & Uhl, C. N. (1983). Single-alternation patterning in a conditioned suppression procedure with and without trace stimulus support: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 21(2) Mar 1983, 157-160.
  • Ayres, J. J., & Vigorito, M. (1984). Posttrial effects of presenting vs. omitting expected shock USs in the conditioned suppression procedure: Concurrent measurement of barpress suppression and freezing: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 12(1) Feb 1984, 73-78.
  • Azorlosa, J. L., & Cicala, G. A. (1986). Blocking of conditioned suppression with 1 or 10 compound trials: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 14(2) May 1986, 163-167.
  • Babbini, M., Gaiardi, M., & Bartoletti, M. (1973). Effects of morphine on a quickly learned conditioned suppression in rats: Psychopharmacologia Vol 33(4) 1973, 329-332.
  • Baeyens, F., Vansteenwegen, D., Beckers, T., Hermans, D., Kerkhof, I., & De Ceulaer, A. (2005). Extinction and renewal of Pavlovian modulation in human sequential Feature Positive discrimination learning: Learning & Memory Vol 12(2) Mar 2005, 178-192.
  • Baeyens, F., Vervliet, B., Vansteenwegen, D., Beckers, T., Hermans, D., & Eelen, P. (2004). Simultaneous and sequential Feature Negative discriminations: Elemental learning and occasion setting in human Pavlovian conditioning: Learning and Motivation Vol 35(2) May 2004, 136-166.
  • Baker, A. G. (1976). Learned irrelevance and learned helplessness: Rats learn that stimuli, reinforcers, and responses are uncorrelated: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 2(2) Apr 1976, 130-141.
  • Baker, A. G. (1977). Conditioned inhibition arising from a between-sessions negative correlation: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 3(2) Apr 1977, 144-155.
  • Baker, A. G., & MacKintosh, N. J. (1977). Excitatory and inhibitory conditioning following uncorrelated presentations of CS and UCS: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 5(3) Aug 1977, 315-319.
  • Baker, A. G., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1979). Preexposure to the CS alone, US alone, or CS and US uncorrelated: Latent inhibition, blocking by context or learned irrelevance? : Learning and Motivation Vol 10(3) Aug 1979, 278-294.
  • Baker, A. G., Woods, W., Tait, R., & Gardiner, K. (1986). Punishment suppression: Some effects on alternative behaviour: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 38B(2) May 1986, 191-215.
  • Baker, R. T. (2003). Narcissistic and avoidant personality and thought suppression. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Balaz, M. A., Capra, S., Kasprow, W. J., & Miller, R. R. (1982). Latent inhibition of the conditioning context: Further evidence of contextual potentiation of retrieval in the absence of appreciable context-US associations: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 10(2) May 1982, 242-248.
  • Balaz, M. A., Gutsin, P., Cacheiro, H., & Miller, R. R. (1982). Blocking as a retrieval failure: Reactivation of associations to a blocked stimulus: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 34B(2) May 1982, 99-113.
  • Balaz, M. A., Kasprow, W. J., & Miller, R. R. (1982). Blocking with a single compound trial: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 10(3) Aug 1982, 271-276.
  • Baldwin, B. A., & Stephens, D. B. (1973). The effects of conditioned behaviour and environmental factors on plasma corticosteroid levels in pigs: Physiology & Behavior Vol 10(2) Feb 1973, 267-274.
  • Barbaree, H. E., & Weisman, R. G. (1978). Acceleration and suppression of rats' responding to avoid foot shock and tail shock: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 29(2) Mar 1978, 243-254.
  • Barnes, G. R., & Paige, G. D. (2004). Anticipatory VOR Suppression Induced by Visual and Nonvisual Stimuli in Humans: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 92(3) Sep 2004, 1501-1511.
  • Barnet, R. C. (1995). Mediation of second-order excitation by a backward conditioned inhibitor. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Barnet, R. C., Grahame, N. J., & Miller, R. R. (1991). Comparing the magnitudes of second-order conditioning and sensory preconditioning effects: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 29(2) Mar 1991, 133-135.
  • Barnet, R. C., Grahame, N. J., & Miller, R. R. (1993). Temporal encoding as a determinant of blocking: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 19(4) Oct 1993, 327-341.
  • Barnet, R. C., Grahame, N. J., & Miller, R. R. (1995). Trial spacing effects in Pavlovian conditioning: A role for local context: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 23(3) Aug 1995, 340-348.
  • Barnet, R. C., & Miller, R. R. (1996). Second-order excitation mediated by a backward conditioned inhibitor: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 22(3) Jul 1996, 279-296.
  • Barnet, R. C., & Miller, R. R. (1996). Temporal encoding as a determinant of inhibitory control: Learning and Motivation Vol 27(1) Feb 1996, 73-91.
  • Barrett, J. E. (1975). The Estes-Skinner procedure: Inadequacy of traditional interpretations: Psychological Record Vol 25(2) Spr 1975, 167-172.
  • Barrett, J. E. (1983). Interrelationships between behavior and pharmacology as factors determining the effects of nicotine: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 19(6) Dec 1983, 1027-1029.
  • Batson, J. D., & Best, M. R. (1981). Single-element assessment of conditioned inhibition: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 18(6) Dec 1981, 328-330.
  • Beavers, W. O., & Perkins, C. C. (1977). Punishment of nonspecific responses: Does the negative half of the law of effect apply? : Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 9(1) Jan 1977, 14-16.
  • Bedard, A.-C., Ickowicz, A., Logan, G. D., Hogg-Johnson, S., Schachar, R., & Tannock, R. (2003). Selective Inhibition in Children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Off and On Stimulant Medication: Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Vol 31(3) Jun 2003, 315-327.
  • Ben-Eliyahu, S., Yirmiya, R., Shavit, Y., & Liebeskind, J. C. (1990). Stress-induced suppression of natural killer cell cytotoxicity in the rat: A naltrexone-insensitive paradigm: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 104(1) Feb 1990, 235-238.
  • Bennett, C. H., Wills, S. J., Wells, J. O., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1994). Reduced generalization following preexposure: Latent inhibition of common elements or a difference in familiarity? : Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 20(3) Jul 1994, 232-239.
  • Berk, A. M. (1985). Juvenile retention deficits for a conditioned emotional response in rats: Implications for infantile amnesia: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Berk, A. M., & Miller, R. R. (1978). LiCl-induced aversions to audiovisual cues as a function of response measure and CS-US interval: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 24(2) Oct 1978, 185-208.
  • Bernstein, I. L., & Goehler, L. E. (1983). Chronic lithium chloride infusions: Conditioned suppression of food intake and preference: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 97(2) Apr 1983, 290-298.
  • Bernstein, I. L., Treneer, C. M., Goehler, L. E., & Murowchick, E. (1985). Tumor growth in rats: Conditioned suppression of food intake and preference: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 99(5) Oct 1985, 818-830.
  • Berthier, N. E., & Moore, J. W. (1980). Disrupted conditioned inhibition of the rabbit nictitating membrane response following mesencephalic lesions: Physiology & Behavior Vol 25(5) Nov 1980, 667-673.
  • Bertsch, G. J. (1976). Punishment of consummatory and instrumental behavior: A review: Psychological Record Vol 26(1) Win 1976, 13-31.
  • Best, M. R. (1975). Conditioned and latent inhibition in taste-aversion learning: Clarifying the role of learned safety: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 1(2) Apr 1975, 97-113.
  • Best, M. R., & et al. (1985). Extinguishing conditioned inhibition in flavour-aversion learning: Effects of repeated testing and extinction of the excitatory element: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 37B(4) Nov 1985, 359-378.
  • Bevins, R. A., & Ayres, J. J. (1991). Two issues in Pavlovian fear conditioning: Selective fear of bright vs. dark, and CS determinants of CR form: Behavioural Processes Vol 24(3) Nov 1991, 211-218.
  • Bevins, R. A., & Ayres, J. J. (1992). Rats' location during conditioned suppression training: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 20(1) Feb 1992, 8-16.
  • Bevins, R. A., & Ayres, J. J. B. (1994). Factors affecting rats' location during conditioned suppression training: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 22(3) Aug 1994, 302-308.
  • Beylin, A. V., & Shors, T. J. (1998). Stress enhances excitatory trace eyeblink conditioning and opposes acquisition of inhibitory conditioning: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 112(6) Dec 1998, 1327-1338.
  • Bice, A. S., & de Lorge, J. (1978). Response suppression during stimuli temporally different from the prefood stimulus: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 11(6) Jun 1978, 377-379.
  • Blackman, D. E., & Scruton, P. (1973). Conditioned suppression and discriminative control of behavior: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 1(2) May 1973, 90-92.
  • Blackman, D. E., & Scruton, P. (1973). Conditioned suppression of counting behavior in rats: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 19(1) Jan 1973, 93-100.
  • Blair, N. J. (2002). Conditioned inhibition and attention in human associative learning. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Blaisdell, A. P., Bristol, A. S., Gunther, L. M., & Miller, R. R. (1998). Overshadowing and latent inhibition counteract each other: Support for the comparator hypothesis: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 24(3) Jul 1998, 335-351.
  • Blaisdell, A. P., Denniston, J. C., & Miller, R. R. (1997). Unblocking with qualitative change of unconditioned stimulus: Learning and Motivation Vol 28(2) May 1997, 268-279.
  • Blaisdell, A. P., Denniston, J. C., & Miller, R. R. (1998). Temporal encoding as a determinant of overshadowing: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 24(1) Jan 1998, 72-83.
  • Blaisdell, A. P., Denniston, J. C., & Miller, R. R. (1999). Posttraining shifts in the overshadowing stimulus-unconditioned stimulus interval alleviates the overshadowing deficit: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 25(1) Jan 1999, 18-27.
  • Blaisdell, A. P., & Miller, R. R. (2001). Conditioned inhibition produced by extinction-mediated recovery from the relative stimulus validity effect: A test of acquisition and performance models of empirical retrospective revaluation: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 27(1) Jan 2001, 48-58.
  • Blaisdell, A. P., Savastano, H. I., & Miller, R. R. (1999). Overshadowing of explicitly unpaired conditioned inhibition is disrupted by preexposure to the overshadowed inhibitor: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 27(3) Aug 1999, 346-357.
  • Blake, R., Cool, S. J., & Crawford, M. L. (1974). Visual resolution in the cat: Vision Research Vol 14(11) Nov 1974, 1211-1217.
  • Blankenship, M. R., Finn, P. R., & Steinmetz, J. E. (1998). A characterization of approach and avoidance learning in alcohol-preferring and alcohol-nonpreferring rats: Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research Vol 22(6) Sep 1998, 1227-1233.
  • Blazis, D. E., & Moore, J. W. (1982). Naloxone does not impair conditioned inhibition of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 20(2) Aug 1982, 122-123.
  • Blazis, D. E., & Moore, J. W. (1991). Conditioned inhibition of the nictitating membrane response in rabbits following hypothalamic and mesencephalic lesions: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 46(1) Dec 1991, 71-81.
  • Blythe, B. L. (1986). Stimulus variables influencing conditioned suppression of copulatory behavior in male rats: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Bolles, R. C., Collier, A. C., Bouton, M. E., & Marlin, N. A. (1978). Some tricks for ameliorating the trace-conditioning deficit: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 11(6) Jun 1978, 403-406.
  • Bolles, R. C., Holtz, R., Dunn, T., & Hill, W. (1980). Comparisons of stimulus learning and response learning in a punishment situation: Learning and Motivation Vol 11(1) Feb 1980, 78-96.
  • Bolles, R. C., Uhl, C. N., Wolfe, M., & Chase, P. B. (1975). Stimulus learning versus response learning in a discriminated punishment situation: Learning and Motivation Vol 6(4) Nov 1975, 439-447.
  • Bonardi, C. (1988). Mechanisms of inhibitory discriminative control: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 16(4) Nov 1988, 445-450.
  • Bond, N. W. (1979). Conditioned suppression, heart rate and pulse volume: Effects of instructions on extinction: Physiology & Behavior Vol 23(5) Nov 1979, 839-843.
  • Bond, N. W., & Blackman, D. E. (1975). Conditioned suppression of schedule-induced polydipsia in rats: Psychological Reports Vol 37(1) Aug 1975, 63-68.
  • Bond, N. W., Blackman, D. E., & Scruton, P. (1973). Suppression of operant behavior and schedule-induced licking in rats: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 20(3) Nov 1973, 375-383.
  • Borg, E., & Engstrom, B. (1983). Hearing thresholds in the rabbit: A behavioral and electrophysiological study: Acta Oto-Laryngologica Vol 95(1-2) Jan-Feb 1983, 19-26.
  • Bormann, N. M. (1995). The effectiveness of morphine as a conditioned stimulus in a conditioned suppression of drinking paradigm. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Bormann, N. M., & Overton, D. A. (1993). Morphine as a conditioned stimulus in a conditioned emotional response paradigm: Psychopharmacology Vol 112(2-3) Sep 1993, 277-284.
  • Boroczi, G., Storms, L. H., & Broer, W. E. (1964). Response suppression and recovery of responding at different deprivation levels as functions of intensity and duration of punishment: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 58(3) Dec 1964, 456-459.
  • Borrero, J. C., Vollmer, T. R., & Wright, C. (2002). An evaluation of contingency strength and response suppression: Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis Vol 35(4) Win 2002, 337-347.
  • Bottjer, S. W. (1980). Extinction and "disinhibition" of conditioned excitation and inhibition: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Bouton, M. E. (1984). Differential control by context in the inflation and reinstatement paradigms: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 10(1) Jan 1984, 56-74.
  • Bouton, M. E. (1986). Slow reacquisition following the extinction of conditioned suppression: Learning and Motivation Vol 17(1) Feb 1986, 1-15.
  • Bouton, M. E., & Bolles, R. C. (1980). Conditioned fear assessed by freezing and by the suppression of three different baselines: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 8(3) Aug 1980, 429-434.
  • Bouton, M. E., & King, D. A. (1986). Effect of context on performance to conditioned stimuli with mixed histories of reinforcement and nonreinforcement: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 12(1) Jan 1986, 4-15.
  • Bouton, M. E., & Ricker, S. T. (1994). Renewal of extinguished responding in a second context: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 22(3) Aug 1994, 317-324.
  • Bouton, M. E., & Swartzentruber, D. (1986). Analysis of the associative and occasion-setting properties of contexts participating in a Pavlovian discrimination: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 12(4) Oct 1986, 333-350.
  • Bovbjerg, D., Kim, Y. T., & Siskind, G. W. (1987). Conditioned suppression of plaque-forming cell responses with cyclophosphamide: The role of taste aversion. New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences.
  • Bovbjerg, D. H. (1983). Classically conditioned alterations in two cell-mediated immune responses: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Bowman, M. T. (1994). The role of preexposure of single and compound flavor stimuli in the development of conditioned inhibition to a flavor compound. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Boyd, T. L., Callen, E. J., & House, W. J. (1989). The effects of post-stress exposure to alcohol upon the development of alcohol consumption in rats: Behaviour Research and Therapy Vol 27(1) 1989, 35-41.
  • Branch, M. N., Nicholson, G., & Dworkin, S. I. (1977). Punishment-specific effects of pentobarbital: Dependency on the type of punisher: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 28(3) Nov 1977, 285-293.
  • Brebner, K., Froestl, W., & Roberts, D. C. S. (2002). The GABA-sub(B ) antagonist CGP56433A attenuates the effect of baclofen on cocaine but not heroin self-administration in the rat: Psychopharmacology Vol 160(1) Feb 2002, 49-55.
  • Brennan, J. F. (1979). Prefrontal lesion effects on suppression of open-field activity in young and adult rats: Physiological Psychology Vol 7(4) Dec 1979, 333-338.
  • Brennan, J. F., Byrnes, J. J., & Jastreboff, P. J. (1993). Salicylate-induced phantom auditory effects on reinforced behavior in pre- and postweanling rats: Psychobiology Vol 21(1) Mar 1993, 60-68.
  • Brennan, J. F., & Jastreboff, P. J. (1991). Generalization of conditioned suppression during salicylate-induced phantom auditory perception in rats: Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis Vol 51(1-2) 1991, 15-27.
  • Brennan, J. F., & Riccio, D. C. (1975). Stimulus generalization of suppression in rats following aversively motivated instrumental or Pavlovian training: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 88(2) Feb 1975, 570-579.
  • Brigui, N., le Bourg, E., & Medioni, J. (1990). Conditioned suppression of the proboscis-extension response in young, middle-aged, and old Drosophila melanogaster flies: Acquisition and extinction: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 104(3) Sep 1990, 289-296.
  • Bristol, A. S., & Carew, T. J. (2005). Differential role of inhibition in habituation of two independent afferent pathways to a common motor output: Learning & Memory Vol 12(1) Jan 2005, 52-60.
  • Britton, G. B. (2001). Neuronal activity in cerebellar interpositus nucleus and red nucleus during conditioned inhibition of the eyeblink response. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Brodigan, D. L., & Trapold, M. A. (1974). Recovery from conditioned suppression to a partially overlapping compound stimulus: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 2(2) May 1974, 89-91.
  • Brody, J. F., Jr. (1966). Conditioned suppression maintained by loud noise instead of shock: Psychonomic Science Vol 6(1) 1966, 27-28.
  • Brown, C. P. (1977). Development of passive avoidance in young chicks: Dependence on inhibition: Psychological Reports Vol 41(3, Pt 2) Dec 1977, 1123-1129.
  • Brown, G. D. (1998). Nonassociative learning processes affecting swimming probability in the seaslug Tritonia diomedea: Habituation, sensitization and inhibition: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 95(2) Oct 1998, 151-165.
  • Brown-Su, A. M., Matzel, L. D., Gordon, E. L., & Miller, R. R. (1986). Malleability of conditioned associations: Path dependence: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 12(4) Oct 1986, 420-427.
  • Bruhwyler, J., Chleide, E., Gueur, H., & Mercier, M. (1988). Transformation of a waiting schedule into a temporal regulation schedule (DRRD) by addition of external stimuli in the dog: Behavioural Processes Vol 17(2) Aug 1988, 117-129.
  • Bruno, A., Brambati, S. M., Perani, D., & Morrone, M. C. (2006). Development of Saccadic Suppression in Children: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 96(3) Sep 2006, 1011-1017.
  • Brush, F. R., & Gendron, C. M. (1998). On the development of inhibition of delay by rats of the Syracuse high- and low-avoidance strains: Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis Vol 58(2) 1998, 123-129.
  • Brutkowski, S., & Dabrowska, J. (1965). Temporal factors in differentiation of instrumental conditioning variety: Acta Biologiae Experimentalis 25(4) 1965, 359-362.
  • Bryant, R. A., & Kourch, M. (2001). Hypnotically induced emotional numbing: International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis Vol 49(3) Jul 2001, 220-230.
  • Bucci, D. J., Hopkins, M. E., Keene, C. S., Sharma, M., & Orr, L. E. (2008). Sex differences in learning and inhibition in spontaneously hypertensive rats: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 187(1) Feb 2008, 27-32.
  • Bucknam, W. (2007). Suppression of symptoms of alcohol dependence and craving using high-dose Baclofen: Alcohol and Alcoholism Vol 42(2) Mar-Apr 2007, 158-160.
  • Buckolz, E., Hall, C., & Alain, C. (1982). Suppressing response initiation in a choice RT task: Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences Vol 7(1) Mar 1982, 49-54.
  • Burdick, C. K., & James, J. P. (1973). Effects of a backward conditioning procedure following acquisition on extinction of conditioned suppression: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 1(2) May 1973, 137-139.
  • Burger, D. C., Denniston, J. C., & Miller, R. R. (2001). Temporal coding in conditioned inhibition: Retardation tests: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 29(3) Aug 2001, 281-290.
  • Burger, D. C., Denniston, J. C., & Miller, R. R. (2001). "Temporal coding in conditioned inhibition: Retardation tests": Erratum: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 29(4) Nov 2001, 388.
  • Burger, D. C., Mallemat, H., & Miller, R. R. (2000). Overshadowing of subsequent events and recovery thereafter: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 53B(2) May 2000, 149-171.
  • Burkhardt, P. E. (1980). One-trial backward fear conditioning in rats as a function of US intensity: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 15(1) Jan 1980, 9-11.
  • Burkhardt, P. E., & Ayres, J. J. (1978). CS and US duration effects in one-trial simultaneous fear conditioning as assessed by conditioned suppression of licking in rats: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 6(2) May 1978, 225-230.
  • Busch, N. A., Groh-Bordin, C., Zimmer, H. D., & Herrmann, C. S. (2008). Modes of memory: Early electrophysiological markers of repetition suppression and recognition enhancement predict behavioral performance: Psychophysiology Vol 45(1) Jan 2008, 25-35.
  • Byrd, J. I. (1975). The effects of prior social experiences on the acquisition and persistence of instrumental behavior: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Byrne, R. M. J., Espino, O., & Santamaria, C. (1999). Counterexamples and the suppression of interferences: Journal of Memory and Language Vol 40(3) Apr 1999, 347-373.
  • Caiazzo, A. J., Hertzberg, H., & Danto, J. (1977). Behavioral thresholds of the guinea pig with the conditioned suppression technique: Journal of Auditory Research Vol 17(2) Apr 1977, 93-98.
  • Caine, S. B., Geyer, M. A., & Swerdlow, N. R. (1995). Effects of D-sub-3/D-sub-2 dopamine receptor agonists and antagonists on prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in the rat: Neuropsychopharmacology Vol 12(2) Apr 1995, 139-145.
  • Calton, J. L. (1997). Conditioned inhibition produced by extinction of a conditioned stimulus. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Calton, J. L., Mitchell, K. G., & Schachtman, T. R. (1996). Conditioned inhibition produced by extinction of a conditioned stimulus: Learning and Motivation Vol 27(4) Nov 1996, 335-361.
  • Cameron, L. N. (1977). The efficacy of conditioning resulting when a classically conditioned punishing reaction is established by the differential pairing of internal and external CS's with 5 US's: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Campolattaro, M. M., & Freeman, J. H. (2006). Perirhinal cortex lesions impair feature-negative discrimination: Neurobiology of Learning and Memory Vol 86(2) Sep 2006, 205-213.
  • Candido, A., Gonzalez, F., & de Brugada, I. (2004). Safety signals from avoidance learning but not from yoked classical conditioning training pass both summation and retardation tests for inhibition: Behavioural Processes Vol 66(2) May 2004, 153-160.
  • Canli, T., & Donegan, N. H. (1995). Conditioned diminution of the unconditioned response in rabbit eyeblink conditioning: Identifying neural substrates in the cerebellum and brainstem: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 109(5) Oct 1995, 874-892.
  • Cappeliez, P., & Moore, E. (1988). Effects of lithium on latent inhibition in the rat: Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry Vol 12(4) 1988, 431-443.
  • Carder, B., & Cheng, R. S. (1976). Behavioral disinhibition by mescaline: Life Sciences Vol 18(6) Mar 1976, 585-591.
  • Carr, A. F. (1974). Latent inhibition and overshadowing in conditioned emotional response conditioning with rats: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 86(4) Apr 1974, 718-723.
  • Carr, J. E., Bailey, J. S., Ecott, C. L., Lucker, K. D., & Weil, T. M. (1998). On the effects of noncontingent delivery of differing magnitudes of reinforcement: Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis Vol 31(3) Fal 1998, 313-321.
  • Carretti, B., Cornoldi, C., & Pelegrina, S. L. (2007). Which factors influence number updating in working memory? The effects of size distance and suppression: British Journal of Psychology Vol 98(1) Feb 2007, 45-60.
  • Carver, A. C., Livesey, D. J., & Charles, M. (2001). Age related changes in inhibitory control as measured by stop signal task performance: International Journal of Neuroscience Vol 107(1-2) Mar 2001, 43-61.
  • Carver, A. C., Livesey, D. J., & Charles, M. (2001). Further manipulation of the stop-signal task: Developmental changes in the ability to inhibit responding with longer stop-signal delays: International Journal of Neuroscience Vol 111(1-2) 2001, 39-53.
  • Cassaday, H. J., Hodges, H., & Gray, J. A. (1993). The effects of ritanserin, RU 24969 and 8-OH-DPAT on latent inhibition in the rat: Journal of Psychopharmacology Vol 7(1) 1993, 63-71.
  • Cassaday, H. J., Mitchell, S. N., Williams, J. H., & Gray, J. A. (1993). 5,7-Dihydroxytryptamine lesions in the fornix-fimbria attenuate latent inhibition: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 59(3) May 1993, 194-207.
  • Cassaday, H. J., Mitchell, S. N., Williams, J. H., & Gray, J. A. (1993). 5,7-Dihydroxytryptamine lesions in the fornix-fimbria attenuate latent inhibition": Erratum: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 60(2) Sep 1993, 186.
  • Cassaday, H. J., Shilliam, C. S., & Marsden, C. A. (2001). Serotonergic depletion increases conditioned suppression to background stimuli in the rat: Journal of Psychopharmacology Vol 15(2) 2001, 83-92.
  • Caul, W. F., Fernandez, K., & Michaelis, R. C. (1983). Effects of prenatal ethanol exposure on heart rate, activity, and response suppression: Neurobehavioral Toxicology & Teratology Vol 5(4) Jul-Aug 1983, 461-464.
  • Caul, W. F., Freeman, B. J., & Buchanan, D. C. (1975). Effects of differential rearing condition on heart rate conditioning and response suppression: Developmental Psychobiology Vol 8(1) Jan 1975, 63-68.
  • Chan, K. H., Jarrard, L. E., & Davidson, T. L. (2003). The effects of selective ibotenate lesions of the hippocampus on conditioned inhibition and extinction: Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 3(2) Jun 2003, 111-119.
  • Chan, K. H., Morell, J. R., Jarrard, L. E., & Davidson, T. L. (2001). Reconsideration of the role of the hippocampus in learned inhibition: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 119(2) Mar 2001, 111-130.
  • Chandler, B. J. (1977). The effect of preacquisition shocks on the development of Pavlovian excitation and inhibition: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Chen, R., Yung, D., & Li, J.-Y. (2003). Organization of Ipsilateral Excitatory and Inhibitory Pathways in the Human Motor Cortex: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 89(3) Mar 2003, 1256-1264.
  • Cherkin, A. (1972). Retrograde amnesia in the chick: Resistance to the reminder effect: Physiology & Behavior Vol 8(5) May 1972, 949-955.
  • Chevalier, J. A. (1965). Inhibition of response to foot shock through competition with a concurrent conditioned response: Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association 1965, 133-134.
  • Chiang, T.-C., Walsh, V., & Lavidor, M. (2004). The cortical representation of foveal stimuli: Evidence from quadrantanopia and TMS-induced suppression: Cognitive Brain Research Vol 21(3) Nov 2004, 309-316.
  • Chiarello, C., & Maxfield, L. (1996). Varieties of interhemispheric inhibition: or How to keep a good hemisphere down: Brain and Cognition Vol 30(1) Feb 1996, 81-108.
  • Chiba, S., & Ando, K. (1976). Effects of chronic administration of kanamycin on conditioned suppression to auditory stimulus in rats: Japanese Journal of Pharmacology Vol 26(4) Aug 1976, 419-426.
  • Christison, G. W., Atwater, G. E., Dunn, L. A., & Kilts, C. D. (1988). Haloperidol enhancement of latent inhibition: Relation to therapeutic action? : Biological Psychiatry Vol 23(7) Apr 1988, 746-749.
  • Cicala, G. A., & Azorlosa, J. L. (1985). Stimulus specificity in avoidance learning: Learning and Motivation Vol 16(1) Feb 1985, 83-94.
  • Cichelli, M. J., & Lewis, M. J. (2002). Naloxone nonselective suppression of drinking of ethanol, sucrose, saccharin and water by rats: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 72(3) Jun 2002, 699-706.
  • Coburn, H. E. (1953). The brain analogy: association tracts: Psychological Review Vol 60(3) May 1953, 197-206.
  • Cohen, N., Ader, R., Green, N., & Bovbjerg, D. (1979). Conditioned suppression of a thymus-independent antibody response: Psychosomatic Medicine Vol 41(6) Oct 1979, 487-491.
  • Cohen, S. L., Knight, M., Tamminga, C. A., & Chase, T. N. (1983). Minimized tolerance to the suppressive effects of CCK8 on operant responding: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 39(1) Sep 1983, 123-127.
  • Coile, D. C., Pollitz, C. H., & Smith, J. C. (1989). Behavioral determination of critical flicker fusion in dogs: Physiology & Behavior Vol 45(6) Jun 1989, 1087-1092.
  • Cole, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (1987). Dissociable effects of lesions to the dorsal or ventral noradrenergic bundle on the acquisition, performance, and extinction of aversive conditioning: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 101(4) Aug 1987, 476-488.
  • Cole, R. P. (1996). Conditioned excitation and conditioned inhibition acquired through backward conditioning. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Cole, R. P., Barnet, R. C., & Miller, R. R. (1995). Effect of relative stimulus validity: Learning or performance deficit? : Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 21(4) Oct 1995, 293-303.
  • Cole, R. P., Barnet, R. C., & Miller, R. R. (1995). Temporal encoding in trace conditioning: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 23(2) May 1995, 144-153.
  • Cole, R. P., Barnet, R. C., & Miller, R. R. (1997). An evaluation of conditioned inhibition as defined by Rescorla's two-test strategy: Learning and Motivation Vol 28(3) Aug 1997, 323-341.
  • Cole, R. P., Oberling, P., & Miller, R. R. (1999). Recovery from one-trial overshadowing: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 6(3) Sep 1999, 424-431.
  • Coleman, D. A. (1984). Relative CS duration in conditioned suppression: Scheduling effects of the unconditioned stimulus and the operant reinforcer: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Colombo, G., Lobina, C., Maccioni, P., Mascia, M. F., Orru, A., Gessa, G. L., et al. (2005). Suppression of maintenance of alcohol-drinking behavior by the concurrent availability of saccharin in Sardinian alcohol-preferring (sP) rats: Alcohol Vol 35(1) Jan 2005, 35-41.
  • Coltheart, V., Avons, S. E., & Trollope, J. (1990). Articulatory suppression and phonological codes in reading for meaning: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology Vol 42(2-A) May 1990, 375-399.
  • Commissaris, R. L., Harrington, G. M., & Altman, H. J. (1990). Benzodiazepine anti-conflict effects in Maudsley Reactive (MR/Har) and Non-Reactive (MNRA/Har) rats: Psychopharmacology Vol 100(3) Mar 1990, 287-292.
  • Commissaris, R. L., Harrington, G. M., Ortiz, A. M., & Altman, H. J. (1986). Maudsley reactive and non-reactive rat strains: Differential performance in a conflict task: Physiology & Behavior Vol 38(2) 1986, 291-294.
  • Commissaris, R. L., Vasas, R. J., & McCloskey, T. C. (1988). Convulsant versus typical barbiturates: Effects on conflict behavior in the rat: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 29(3) Mar 1988, 631-634.
  • Commissaris, R. L., Verbanac, J. S., Markovska, V. L., & Altman, H. J. (1996). Anxiety-like and depression-like behavior in Maudsley reactive (MR) and non-reactive (NMRA) rats: Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry Vol 20(3) Apr 1996, 491-5-1.
  • Cook, D. A. (1996). Confronting and subduing the beast within: The struggle for the cognitive control of prejudice and the effects of suppressing racist thoughts. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Cooper, L. J., & et al. (1981). Behavioral responses of pigeons to high-intensity 60-Hz electric fields: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 32(2) Jun 1981, 214-228.
  • Cooper, R. M., & Koppenaal, R. J. (1964). Suppression and recovery of a one-trial avoidance response after a single ECS: Psychonomic Science Vol 1(10) 1964, 303-304.
  • Cooper, S. J. (1982). Palatability-induced drinking after administration of morphine, naltrexone and diazepam in the non-deprived rat: Substance & Alcohol Actions/Misuse Vol 3(5) 1982, 259-266.
  • Cooper, S. J. (1983). Suppression of saccharin-induced drinking in the nondeprived rat by low dose diazepam treatment: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 18(5) May 1983, 825-827.
  • Corda, M. G., Giorgi, O., & Biggio, G. (1986). Behavioural and biochemical evidence for a long-lasting decrease in GABAergic function elicited by chronic administration of FG 7142: Brain Research Vol 384(1) Oct 1986, 60-67.
  • Corwin, J. V., & Slaughter, J. S. (1976). Modality and prior habituation as critical factors in development of rapid discriminated conditioned suppression in rats: Psychological Reports Vol 38(3, Pt 2) Jun 1976, 1259-1265.
  • Corwin, J. V., & Slaughter, J. S. (1979). Effects of vagal stimulation on the learning of specific and diffuse conditioned suppression: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 25(3) Mar 1979, 364-370.
  • Costello, N. L., Carlson, J. N., & Glick, S. D. (1991). Acute administration of diazepam and buspirone in rats trained on conflict schedules having different degrees of predictability: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 40(4) Dec 1991, 787-794.
  • Costello, N. L., Carlson, J. N., Glick, S. D., & Bryda, M. (1991). The effects of acute administration of gepirone in rats trained on conflict schedules having different degrees of predictability: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 40(4) Dec 1991, 795-800.
  • Cotter, P. D. (1974). The immediate and enduring effects of punishment as a function of alternative responses, schedule of punishment, and intensity: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Cotton, M. M., Goodall, G., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1982). Inhibitory conditioning resulting from a reduction in the magnitude of reinforcement: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 34B(3) Aug 1982, 163-180.
  • Coulombe, D., & White, N. (1982). Posttraining self-stimulation and memory: A study of some parameters: Physiological Psychology Vol 10(3) Sep 1982, 343-349.
  • Coulon, N., Comblez, V., Lamerand, H., & Toe, P. (1995). The effects of age on response inhibition in a Stroop interference paradigm: Bulletin de Psychologie Vol 49(422) Nov-Dec 1995, 38-40.
  • Couvillon, P. A., Ablan, C. D., & Bitterman, M. E. (1999). Exploratory studies of inhibitory conditioning in honeybees (Apis mellifera): Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 25(1) Jan 1999, 103-112.
  • Couvillon, P. A., Hsiung, R., Cooke, A. M., & Bitterman, M. E. (2005). The role of context in the inhibitory conditioning of honeybees: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 58B(1) Jan 2005, 59-67.
  • Crawford, L. L., & Domjan, M. (1996). Conditioned inhibition of social approach in male Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) using visual exposure to a female: Behavioural Processes Vol 36(2) Apr 1996, 163-169.
  • Crow, T., Redell, J. B., Tian, L.-M., Xue-Bian, J., & Dash, P. K. (2003). Inhibition of Conditioned Stimulus Pathway Phosphoprotein 24 Expression Blocks the Development of Intermediate-Term Memory in Hermissenda: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 23(8) Apr 2003, 3415-3422.
  • Crow, T. J. (1974). A behavioral and electrophysiological analysis of cortical spreading depression: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Cunningham, C. L. (1981). Association between the elements of a bivalent compound stimulus: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 7(4) Oct 1981, 425-436.
  • Dafters, R. I. (1976). Effect of medial archistriatal lesions on the conditioned emotional response and on auditory discrimination performance of the pigeon: Physiology & Behavior Vol 17(4) Oct 1976, 659-665.
  • Dahmen, K. (1986). An experimental comparison of the effects of discriminative punishment and conditioned suppression in kindergarteners: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie Vol 33(2) 1986, 194-207.
  • Dantas Ferrara, M. L., Alves Banaco, R., Wielenska, R. C., & Azzi, R. G. (1981). Conditional suppression: A study on the relation between signal localization and reinforcement frequency: Psicologia Vol 7(2) Jul 1981, 39-50.
  • Dantzer, R., & Baldwin, B. A. (1974). Changes in heart rate during suppression of operant responding in pigs: Physiology & Behavior Vol 12(3) Mar 1974, 385-391.
  • Dantzer, R., & Baldwin, B. A. (1974). Effects of chlordiazepoxide on heart rate and behavioural suppression in pigs subjected to operant conditioning procedures: Psychopharmacologia Vol 37(2) 1974, 169-177.
  • Dantzer, R., & Delacour, J. (1972). Modification of a phenomenon of conditioned suppression by a thalamic lesion: Physiology & Behavior Vol 8(6) Jun 1972, 997-1003.
  • Dantzer, R., & Roca, M. (1974). Tranquilizing effects of diazepam in pigs subjected to a punishment procedure: Psychopharmacologia Vol 40(3) 1974, 235-240.
  • Das, J. P., & Papadopoulos, T. C. (2003). Behavioural inhibition and hyperactivity: A commentary from alternative perspectives: European Journal of Special Needs Education Vol 18(2) Jun 2003, 183-195.
  • Davidson, T. L. (1990). The long-term effects of diazepam, lorazepam, and buspirone on behavioral suppression by a shock signal: Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry Vol 14(2) 1990, 223-236.
  • Davidson, T. L., & Lucki, I. (1987). The long-term effects of diazepam and pentylenetetrazol on behavioral sensitivity to a stressor: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 27(1) May 1987, 99-103.
  • Davis, H., Green, B., Herrmann, T., & Levine, S. (1978). Blockage of pituitary-adrenal activity does not affect conditioned suppression: Physiology & Behavior Vol 20(4) Apr 1978, 423-425.
  • Davis, H., Herrmann, T., & Shattuck, D. (1979). Summation of excitatory and inhibitory control produced by traditional tone-shock contingencies and autocontingencies: Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science Vol 14(4) Oct-Dec 1979, 254-262.
  • Davis, H., & Levine, S. (1982). Predictability, control, and the pituitary-adrenal response in rats: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 96(3) Jun 1982, 393-404.
  • Davis, H., & MacFadden, L. (1978). Is autocontingency control established when a traditional contingency is simultaneously available? : Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 11(6) Jun 1978, 387-389.
  • Davis, H., & Memmott, J. (1984). Autocontingencies: Suppressive and accelerative effects of pairs of shocks superimposed on a positively reinforced operant baseline: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 42(1) Jul 1984, 75-86.
  • Davis, H., Memmott, J., & Hurwitz, H. M. (1976). Effects of signals preceding and following shock on baseline responding during a conditioned-suppression procedure: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 25(2) Mar 1976, 263-277.
  • Davis, H., & Shattuck, D. (1980). Transfer of conditioned suppression and conditioned acceleration from instrumental to consummatory baselines: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 8(2) May 1980, 253-257.
  • Davis, H., Shattuck, D., & Wright, J. (1981). Autocontingencies: Factors underlying control of operant baselines by compound tone/shock/no-shock contingencies: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 9(3) Aug 1981, 322-331.
  • Davis, H., & Wright, J. (1979). Procedural and parametric variability in studies of conditioned suppression: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 14(3) Sep 1979, 149-150.
  • Davis, H. P., Rosenzweig, M. R., Jones, O. W., & Bennett, E. L. (1981). Inhibition of cerebral protein synthesis does not prolong short-term memory: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 95(4) Aug 1981, 556-564.
  • Davis, M., Falls, W. A., & Gewirtz, J. (2000). Neural systems involved in fear inhibition: Extinction and conditioned inhibition. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
  • Davis, N. M., & Gray, J. A. (1983). Brain 5-hydroxytryptamine and learned resistance to punishment: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 8(2) May 1983, 129-137.
  • Davis, R. E. (1981). A method of restraining goldfish during conditioning of electric-shock-induced suppression of breathing movements: Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation Vol 13(5) Oct 1981, 650-652.
  • Davis, W. J., & Gillette, R. (1978). Neural correlate of behavioral plasticity in command neurons of Pleurobranchaea: Science Vol 199(4330) Feb 1978, 801-804.
  • Davison, M., Sheldon, L., & Lobb, B. (1980). Positive conditioned suppression: Transfer of performance between contingent and non-contingent reinforcement situations: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 33(1) Jan 1980, 51-57.
  • De La Casa, G., & Lubow, R. E. (1995). Latent inhibition in conditioned taste aversion: The roles of stimulus frequency and duration and the amount of fluid ingested during preexposure: Neurobiology of Learning and Memory Vol 64(2) Sep 1995, 125-132.
  • De la Casa, L. G., & Lubow, R. E. (2002). An empirical analysis of the super-latent inhibition effect: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 30(2) May 2002, 112-120.
  • De Toledo, L., & Black, A. H. (1966). Heart rate: Changes during conditioned suppression in rats: Science 152(3727) 1966, 1404-1406.
  • Dearing, M. F., & Dickinson, A. (1979). Counterconditioning of shock by a water reinforcer in rabbits: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 7(3) Aug 1979, 360-366.
  • DeJianne, D., McGuire, T. R., & Pruzan-Hotchkiss, A. (1985). Conditioned suppression of proboscis extension in Drosophila melanogaster: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 99(1) Mar 1985, 74-80.
  • Delamater, A. R., Kruse, J. M., Marlin, S., & LoLordo, V. M. (1986). Conditioned inhibition in taste aversion learning: Testing methodology and empirical status: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 14(1) Feb 1986, 6-14.
  • Delamater, A. R., LoLordo, V. M., & Sosa, W. (2003). Outcome-specific conditioned inhibition in Pavlovian backward conditioning: Learning & Behavior Vol 31(4) Nov 2003, 393-402.
  • Delfini, L. F. (1975). The effects of intravenous and subcutaneous epinephrine administration on conditioned suppression: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Delprato, D. J., & Jackson, D. E. (1973). Counterconditioning and exposure-only in the treatment of specific (conditioned suppression) and generalized fear in rats: Behaviour Research and Therapy Vol 11(4) Nov 1973, 453-461.
  • Deni, R., & Vocino, J. (1980). Changes in maternal behavior in rats during exposure to a stimulus predicting shock: Psychological Record Vol 30(4) Fal 1980, 525-529.
  • Denis, P. (2003). What kind of latency period for today children? : Neuropsychiatrie de l'Enfance et de l'Adolescence Vol 51(6) Oct 2003, 288-291.
  • Denisova, Z. V. (1964). Interaction of signal systems in preschool children during formation of conditioned responses to object stimuli: Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel'nosti 14(3) 1964, 427-435.
  • Denniston, J. C., Blaisdell, A. P., & Miller, R. R. (1998). Temporal coding affects transfer of serial and simultaneous inhibitors: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 26(3) Aug 1998, 336-350.
  • Denniston, J. C., Cole, R. P., & Miller, R. R. (1998). The role of temporal relationships in the transfer of conditioned inhibition: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 24(2) Apr 1998, 200-214.
  • Denniston, J. C., & Miller, R. R. (2007). Timing of omitted events: An analysis of temporal control of inhibitory behavior: Behavioural Processes Vol 74(2) Feb 2007, 274-285.
  • Desbien, D., & Yelen, D. (1980). Task difficulty and conditioned suppression in humans: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 16(3) Sep 1980, 197-198.
  • Desiderato, O. (1964). Generalization of conditioned suppression: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 57(3) Jun 1964, 434-437.
  • Desmond, J. E., Romano, A. G., & Moore, J. W. (1980). Amplitude of the rabbit's unconditioned nictitating membrane response in the presence of a conditioned inhibitor: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 8(2) May 1980, 225-230.
  • Dess, N. K., & Soltysik, S. S. (1989). Ontogeny of conditioned inhibition of conditioned respiratory suppression in kittens: Developmental Psychobiology Vol 22(3) Apr 1989, 257-269.
  • Dess, N. K., & Soltysik, S. S. (1993). Associative properties of a conditioned inhibitor as a function of age in kittens: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 21(2) May 1993, 138-144.
  • DeVietti, T. L., & Barrett, O. V. (1986). Latent inhibition: No effect of intertrial interval of the preexposure trials: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 24(6) Nov 1986, 453-455.
  • DeVietti, T. L., Bauste, R. L., Nutt, G., Barrett, O. V., & et al. (1987). Latent inhibition: A trace conditioning phenomenon? : Learning and Motivation Vol 18(2) May 1987, 185-201.
  • DeVito, P. L., & Fowler, H. (1987). Enhancement of conditioned inhibition via an extinction treatment: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 15(4) Nov 1987, 448-454.
  • Di Giusto, J. A., Di Giusto, E. L., & King, M. G. (1974). Heart rate and muscle tension correlates of conditioned suppression in humans: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 103(3) Sep 1974, 515-521.
  • Dickinson, A. (1975). Suppressive and enhancing effects of footshock on food-reinforced operant responding following septal lesions in rats: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 88(2) Feb 1975, 851-861.
  • Dickinson, A. (1976). Appetitive-aversion interactions: Facilitation of aversive conditioning by prior appetitive training in the rat: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 4(4) Nov 1976, 416-420.
  • Dickinson, A. (1977). Appetitive-aversive interactions: Superconditioning of fear by an appetitive CS: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 29(1) Feb 1977, 71-83.
  • Dickinson, A., Colwill, R. M., & Pearce, J. M. (1980). Post-trial stimulation and the acquisition of conditioned suppression in the rat: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 32(1) Feb 1980, 149-158.
  • Dickinson, A., Hall, G., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1976). Surprise and the attenuation of blocking: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 2(4) Oct 1976, 313-322.
  • Dickinson, A., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1979). Reinforcer specificity in the enhancement of conditioning by posttrial surprise: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 5(2) Apr 1979, 162-177.
  • Dickinson, A., Nicholas, D. J., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1983). A re-examination of one-trial blocking in conditioned suppression: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 35(B-1) Feb 1983, 67-79.
  • Dickinson, A., & Pearce, J. M. (1976). Preference and response suppression under different correlations between shock and a positive reinforcer in rats: Learning and Motivation Vol 7(1) Feb 1976, 66-85.
  • Dieussaert, K., De Neys, W., & Schaeken, W. (2005). Suppression and belief revision, two sides of the same coin? : Psychologica Belgica Vol 45(1) 2005, 29-46.
  • Diez-Chamizo, V., Blackman, D. E., & Thomas, G. V. (1983). Effects of conditioned suppression in a discriminated operant: Revista de Psicologia General y Aplicada Vol 38(1) 1983, 67-76.
  • DiGiusto, E. L., & Bond, N. W. (1978). One-trial conditioned suppression: Effects of instructions on extinction: American Journal of Psychology Vol 91(2) Jun 1978, 313-319.
  • Dinc, H. I., & Smith, J. C. (1966). Role of the olfactory bulbs in the detection of ionizing radiation by the rat: Physiology & Behavior 1(2) 1966, 139-144.
  • Dirikx, T., Beckers, T., Muyls, C., Eelen, P., Vansteenwegen, D., Hermans, D., et al. (2007). Differential acquisition, extinction, and reinstatement of conditioned suppression in mice: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 60(10) Oct 2007, 1313-1320.
  • Dirikx, T., Gillard, E., Muyls, C., Beckers, T., Hermans, D., Vansteenwegen, D., et al. (2006). Reinstatement of conditioned suppression in mice: Psychologica Belgica Vol 46(1-2) 2006, 185-195.
  • Divenyi, P. L. (1992). Binaural suppression of nonechoes: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Vol 91(2) Feb 1992, 1078-1084.
  • Doe, N. (2000). The effects of fixed sequence of US intensities and CS-US correlation on licking suppression in rats: Japanese Journal of Animal Psychology Vol 50(2) Dec 2000, 213-220.
  • Doe, N., Nakajima, S., & Tamai, N. (2004). Cross-modal transfer of conditioned suppression in rats: Effects of US intensity and extinction of the initial conditioning task: Learning and Motivation Vol 35(3) Aug 2004, 242-261.
  • Doleys, D. M., & Davidson, R. S. (1974). Repeated suppression and recovery of responding using response-produced electric shock: Psychological Reports Vol 35(3) Dec 1974, 1259-1268.
  • Doleys, D. M., & Davidson, R. S. (1976). Effects of pairing electric shock with reinforcement delivered on a variable-interval schedule: Psychological Reports Vol 39(2) Oct 1976, 483-489.
  • Domjan, M. (1977). Selective suppression of drinking during a limited period following aversive drug treatment in rats: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 3(1) Jan 1977, 66-76.
  • Drexler, E. D. (1974). The measurement of inhibition and inhibitory stimulus control: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Droungas, A., & LoLordo, V. M. (1995). The explicitly unpaired procedure yields conditioned inhibition whether the CS and the US alternate singly or randomly: Learning and Motivation Vol 26(3) Aug 1995, 278-299.
  • Dubin, W. J. (1973). The influence of the similarity of the components of a serial CS on the suppression of consummatory licking in the rat: Dissertation Abstracts International Vol.
  • Dumont, M., Yzerbyt, V. Y., Snyder, M., Mathieu, B., Comblain, C., & Scaillet, N. (2003). Suppression and hypothesis testing: Does suppressing stereotypes during interactions help to avoid confirmation biases? : European Journal of Social Psychology Vol 33(5) Sep-Oct 2003, 659-677.
  • Dunham, P. J. (1978). Changes in unpunished responding during response-contingent punishment: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 6(2) May 1978, 174-180.
  • Dunham, P. J. (1979). On categories of behavior and control groups in multiple-response procedures: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 7(2) May 1979, 273-274.
  • Dunn, L. A. (1994). "The latent inhibition model of schizophrenic attention disorder and of antipsychotic drug action": Comment: Psychopharmacology Vol 116(3) Nov 1994, 381.
  • Dunn, L. A., Atwater, G. E., & Kilts, C. D. (1993). Effects of antipsychotic drugs on latent inhibition: Sensitivity and specificity of an animal behavioral model of clinical drug action: Psychopharmacology Vol 112(2-3) Sep 1993, 315-323.
  • Earhard, B. (1976). Retroactive inhibition: Associative loss or response suppression? : American Journal of Psychology Vol 89(3) Sep 1976, 385-401.
  • Edeline, J.-M., Dutrieux, G., & Neuenschwander-el Massioui, N. (1988). Multiunit changes in hippocampus and medial geniculate body in free-behaving rats during acquisition and retention of a conditioned response to a tone: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 50(1) Jul 1988, 61-79.
  • Edwards, J. D., & Eckerman, D. A. (1979). Effects of diazepam and ethanol alone and in combination on conditioned suppression of key-pecking in the pigeon: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 10(2) Feb 1979, 217-221.
  • Eldridge, G. D. (1993). Topographical features of conditioned inhibition in discriminated autoshaping in pigeons: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Elkins, R. L., Gerardot, R. J., & Hobbs, S. H. (1989). Differences in cyclophosphamide-induced suppression of cricket predation in selectively bred strains of taste-aversion prone and resistant rats: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 103(1) Feb 1989, 112-116.
  • Ellen, P., Makohon, L., & Richardson, W. K. (1978). Response suppression on DRL by rats with septal damage: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 92(3) Jun 1978, 511-521.
  • Ernst, A. J., & Yee, R. W. (1975). A developmental analysis of the relationship between response strength and punishment: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 3(2) May 1975, 107-113.
  • Escobar, M., Arcediano, F., & Miller, R. R. (2003). Latent inhibition in human adults without masking: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition Vol 29(5) Sep 2003, 1028-1040.
  • Espinet, A., Artigas, A. A., & Balleine, B. W. (2008). Inhibitory sensory preconditioning detected with a sodium depletion procedure: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 61(2) Feb 2008, 240-247.
  • Espinet, A., Iraola, J. A., Bennett, C. H., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1995). Inhibitory association between neutral stimuli in flavor- aversion conditioning: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 23(4) Nov 1995, 361-368.
  • Evans, P. L., & Hogg, J. (1981). Towards an assessment of conditioned inhibition in trainable and educable retarded children: Behavior Research of Severe Developmental Disabilities Vol 2(1) Sep 1981, 17-36.
  • Everss, E., & Parra, A. (1998). Inhibitory avoidance with a two-way shuttle-box: Psicothema Vol 10(2) Jul 1998, 387-391.
  • Exton, M. S., Gierse, C., Meier, B., Mosen, M., Xie, Y., Frede, S., et al. (2002). Behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression in the rat is regulated via noradrenaline and beta -adrenoceptors: Journal of Neuroimmunology Vol 131(1-2) Oct 2002, 21-30.
  • Eysenck, H. J. (1964). Personality and reminiscence: An experimental study of the "reactive inhibition" and the "conditioned inhibition" theories: Life Sciences 3(3) 1964, 189-198.
  • Eysenck, H. J. (1965). A three-factor theory of reminiscence: British Journal of Psychology 56(2-3) 1965, 163-181.
  • Falls, W. A., & Davis, M. (1995). Behavioral and physiological analysis of fear inhibition: Extinction and conditioned inhibition. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Publishers.
  • Falls, W. A., & Davis, M. (1995). Lesions of the central nucleus of the amygdala block conditioned excitation, but not conditioned inhibition of fear as measured with the fear-potentiated startle effect: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 109(3) Jun 1995, 379-387.
  • Farley, J., & et al. (1983). Membrane changes in a single photoreceptor cause associative learning in Hermissenda: Science Vol 221(4616) Sep 1983, 1201-1203.
  • Feldman, M. P. (1963). A reconsideration of the extinction hypothesis of warm up in motor behavior: Psychological Bulletin Vol 60(5) Sep 1963, 452-459.
  • Feldon, J., Rawlins, J. N., & Gray, J. A. (1982). Discrimination of response-contingent and response-independent shock by rats: Effects of medial and lateral septal lesions and chlordiazepoxide: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 35(2) Jun 1982, 121-138.
  • Feldon, J., Rawlins, J. N., & Gray, J. A. (1982). Effects of lateral and medial septal lesions on response suppression maintained by response-contingent and response-independent shock: Physiological Psychology Vol 10(1) Mar 1982, 145-152.
  • Feldon, J., & Weiner, I. (1989). Abolition of the acquisition but not the expression of latent inhibition by chlordiazepoxide in rats: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 32(1) Jan 1989, 123-127.
  • Fendt, M. (2000). Expression and conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle after stimulation and blockade of AMPA/Kainate and GABA-sub(A ) receptors in the dorsal periaqueductal gray: Brain Research Vol 880(1-2) Oct 2000, 1-10.
  • Fernberger, S. W. (1929). Review of Lectures on Conditioned Reflexes: Psychological Bulletin Vol 26(6) Jun 1929, 371.
  • Figueiredo, L. C., Santarem, E. M., & Ferrara, M. L. (1983). Suppression and facilitation of operant execution as a function of frequency of unavoidable shocks: Psicologia Vol 9(1) Mar 1983, 35-45.
  • Finch, B. E. (1986). Effects of context on conditioned inhibition in taste aversion: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Fiori, L. M., Barnet, R. C., & Miller, R. R. (1994). Renewal of Pavlovian conditioned inhibition: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 22(1) Feb 1994, 47-52.
  • Flaherty, C. F., Coppotelli, C., Grigson, P. S., Mitchell, C., & Flaherty, J. E. (1995). Investigation of the devaluation interpretation of anticipatory negative contrast: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 21(3) Jul 1995, 229-247.
  • Flanagan-Cato, L. M., Grigson, P. S., & King, J. L. (2001). Estrogen-induced suppression of intake is not mediated by taste aversion in female rats: Physiology & Behavior Vol 72(4) Mar 2001, 549-558.
  • Fletcher, P. J. (1993). A comparison of the effects of dorsal or median raphe injections of 8-OH-DPAT in three operant tasks measuring response inhibition: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 54(2) Apr 1993, 187-197.
  • Flores, P., & Pellon, R. (2000). Antipunishment effects of diazepam on two levels of suppression of schedule-induced drinking in rats: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 67(2) Oct 2000, 207-214.
  • Foley-Jones, C., & White, M. J. (1972). Response suppression, generalization, and "experimental neurosis" in the albino rat: Australian Journal of Psychology Vol 24(3) Dec 1972, 331-334.
  • Follick, M. J., & Knutson, J. F. (1978). Punishment of irritable aggression: Aggressive Behavior Vol 4(1) 1978, 1-17.
  • Fontana, D. J., & Commissaris, R. L. (1988). Effects of acute and chronic imipramine administration on conflict behavior in the rat: A potential "animal model" for the study of panic disorder? : Psychopharmacology Vol 95(2) Jun 1988, 147-150.
  • Fontana, D. J., & Commissaris, R. L. (1989). Effects of cocaine on conflict behavior in the rat: Life Sciences Vol 45(9) 1989, 819-827.
  • Fontana, D. J., Schefke, D. M., & Commissaris, R. L. (1989). Acute versus chronic clonidine treatment effects on conflict behavior in the rat: Behavioural Pharmacology Vol 1(3) Spr 1989-1990, 201-208.
  • Ford, M., Bird, B. L., Newton, F. A., & Sheer, D. (1980). Maintenance and generalization of 40-Hz EEG biofeedback effects: Biofeedback & Self Regulation Vol 5(2) Jun 1980, 193-205.
  • Forster, J., & Liberman, N. (2004). A motivational model of post-suppressional rebound. Hove, England: Psychology Press/Taylor & Francis (UK).
  • Forster, J., & Liberman, N. (2005). How motivational inferences influence post-suppressional rebound. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
  • Fournier, G. J. (1977). The effects of a pre-experimental history on conditioned suppression in humans: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Marshall, P. J., Nichols, K. E., & Ghera, M. M. (2005). Behavioral Inhibition: Linking Biology and Behavior within a Developmental Framework: Annual Review of Psychology Vol 56 2005, 235-262.
  • Fox, R. A., & Daunton, N. G. (1982). Conditioned feeding suppression in rats produced by cross-coupled and simple motions: Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol 53(3) Mar 1982, 218-220.
  • Fox, R. A., & et al. (1984). Off-vertical rotation produces conditioned taste aversion and suppressed drinking in mice: Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine Vol 55(7) Jul 1984, 632-635.
  • France, C. P., Adams, J. U., & Woods, J. H. (1985). Intracerebroventricular drug administration in pigeons: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 23(5) Nov 1985, 731-736.
  • Frank, H., & Frank, M. G. (1983). Inhibition training in wolves and dogs: Behavioural Processes Vol 8(4) Oct 1983, 363-377.
  • Frankel, F. D. (1975). The role of the response-punishment contingency in the suppression of a positively-reinforced operant: Learning and Motivation Vol 6(3) Aug 1975, 385-403.
  • Frankel, P. I. (1997). Control of mentation from within and without: Mood management and thought suppression. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Freeman, B. J., & Liebowitz, J. M. (1974). Response facilitation as a function of short pretimeout stimuli in a conditioned suppression paradigm with humans: Psychological Reports Vol 35(1, Pt 1) Aug 1974, 67-72.
  • Freeman, F. G., Mikulka, P. J., & d'Auteuil, P. (1974). Conditioned suppression of a licking response in rats with hippocampal lesions: Behavioral Biology Vol 12(2) Oct 1974, 257-263.
  • Freeman, J. H., Jr., Halverson, H. E., & Poremba, A. (2005). Differential Effects of Cerebellar Inactivation on Eyeblink Conditioned Excitation and Inhibition: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 25(4) Jan 2005, 889-895.
  • Freeman, J. H., Jr., & Nicholson, D. A. (1999). Neuronal activity in the cerebellar interpositus and lateral pontine nuclei during inhibitory classical conditioning of the eyeblink response: Brain Research Vol 833(2) Jul 1999, 225-233.
  • Fregnac, Y., Grandjean, B., & Gary-Bobo, E. (1982). Perceptual deficits in dark reared cats: The use of conditioned suppression to evaluate psychometric functions: L'annee Psychologique Vol 82(1) 1982, 29-43.
  • Fresquet, N. (1997). Effects of aging on inhibitory learning and short-term memory in Drosophila Melanogaster: International Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 10(2) 1997, 90-109.
  • Frey, P., Eng, S., & Gavin, W. (1972). Conditioned suppression in the gerbil: Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation Vol 4(5) Sep 1972, 245-249.
  • Friedman, B. X., Blaisdell, A. P., Escobar, M., & Miller, R. R. (1998). Comparator mechanisms and conditioned inhibition: Conditioned stimulus preexposure disrupts Pavlovian conditioned inhibition but not explicitly unpaired inhibition: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 24(4) Oct 1998, 453-466.
  • Frysztak, R. J., & Crow, T. (1994). Enhancement of Type B and A photoreceptor inhibitory synaptic connections in conditioned Hermissenda: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 14(3, Pt 1) Mar 1994, 1245-1250.
  • Fuentes, L. J., & Vivas, A. B. (2000). The global precedence effect is not affected in inhibition of return: European Journal of Cognitive Psychology Vol 12(4) Dec 2000, 472-488.
  • Fujii, M. (1981). Second-order conditioning in a licking-based conditioned suppression situation: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 23(3) 1981, 149-159.
  • Fujii, M. (1988). Pavlovian conditioned inhibition in a licking conditioned suppression situation with rats: An examination of the Rescorla-Wagner model: Japanese Journal of Psychonomic Science Vol 6(2) Mar 1988, 95-100.
  • Fujii, M., Asada, M., Takata, N., Yamano, A., & Imada, H. (1989). Measurement of emotional reactivity and association ability of the Tsukuba emotional strains of rats (Rattus norvegicus) in licking and lever-pressing conditioned suppression situations: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 103(1) Mar 1989, 100-108.
  • Fujii, M., Uchida, M., & Imada, H. (1994). Effects of presence and absence of information about shock intensity upon licking suppression in rats: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 36(2) 1994, 65-73.
  • Gaito, J. (1985). The suppression of kindling with low-frequency brain stimulation: Statistical data with duration variable: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 23(4) Jul 1985, 332-334.
  • Gallo, M., & Candido, A. (1995). Dorsal hippocampal lesions impair blocking but not latent inhibition of taste aversion learning in rats: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 109(3) Jun 1995, 413-425.
  • Garrud, P., & et al. (1984). Successful overshadowing and blocking in hippocampectomized rats: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 12(1) Apr 1984, 39-53.
  • Gavrilova, L. N. (1963). Conditioned inhibition in dogs with a unilaterally removed thalamus: Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel'nosti 13(6) 1963, 1039-1046.
  • Geller, I. (1964). Conditioned suppression in goldfish as a function of shock-reinforcement schedule: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 7(5) 1964, 345-350.
  • Gellert, V. F., & Sparber, S. B. (1977). A comparison of the effects of naloxone upon body weight loss and suppression of fixed-ratio operant behavior in morphine-dependent rats: Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics Vol 201(1) Apr 1977, 44-54.
  • Gemberling, G. A., Domjan, M., & Amsel, A. (1980). Aversion learning in 5-day-old rats: Taste-toxicosis and texture-shock associations: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 94(4) Aug 1980, 734-745.
  • Gengerelli, J. A. (1929). Review of Methods for Investigating the Conditioned Reflexes in Man: Psychological Bulletin Vol 26(6) Jun 1929, 372-373.
  • Genty, E., Palmier, C., & Roeder, J.-J. (2004). Learning to suppress responses to the larger of two rewards in two species of lemurs, Eulemur fulvus and E. macaco: Animal Behaviour Vol 67(5) May 2004, 925-932.
  • Germas, J. E. (1975). The effects of low-level, transcranial DC polarization on conditioned suppression in the rat: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Gescheider, G. A., & Verrillo, R. T. (1982). Contralateral enhancement and suppression of vibrotactile sensation: Perception & Psychophysics Vol 32(1) Jul 1982, 69-74.
  • Gewirtz, J. C., & Davis, M. (1995). Habituation of prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex using an auditory prepulse close to background noise: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 109(3) Jun 1995, 388-395.
  • Gewirtz, J. C., Falls, W. A., & Davis, M. (1997). Normal conditioning inhibition and extinction of freezing and fear-potentiated startle following electrolytic lesions of medial prefrontal cortex in rats: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 111(4) Aug 1997, 712-726.
  • Ghezzi, P. M., & Cheney, C. D. (1983). Stimulus and prefood stimulus effects on fixed-interval and fixed-ratio responding: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 21(6) Nov 1983, 491-494.
  • Ghirlanda, S. (2005). Retrospective Revaluation as Simple Associative Learning: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 31(1) Jan 2005, 107-111.
  • Gillen, N., & Keehn, J. D. (1983). Effects of a conditioned suppression procedure on schedule-induced drinking by rats: Psychological Record Vol 33(2) Spr 1983, 269-278.
  • Giordano, M., Salado-Castillo, R., Sanchez-Alavez, M., & Prado-Alcala, R. A. (1998). Striatal transplants prevent AF64A-induced retention deficits: Life Sciences Vol 63(22) Oct 1998, 317-322.
  • Girolametto-Goodison, T. M. (1995). Pavlovian conditioning and tolerance to cholecystokinin-8 and naloxone-induced meal suppression. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Gleitman, H., Nachmias, J., & Neisser, U. (1954). The S-R reinforcement theory of extinction: Psychological Review Vol 61(1) Jan 1954, 23-33.
  • Glowa, J. R., & Barrett, J. E. (1983). Response suppression by visual stimuli paired with postsession d-amphetamine injections in the pigeon: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 39(1) Jan 1983, 165-173.
  • Glowa, J. R., & Insel, T. R. (1992). Effects of chlordiazepoxide and !b-carboline 3-carboxylic acid ethyl ester on non-suppressed and minimally-suppressed responding in the squirrel monkey: Life Sciences Vol 50(1) 1992, 7-14.
  • Glowa, J. R., & Williams, A. N. (1992). Effects of prior exposure to cocaine: Interaction of reinforcing and suppressant effects: Life Sciences Vol 51(13) 1992, 987-994.
  • Goldberg, S. R. (1976). Conditioned behavioral and physiological changes associated with injections of a narcotic antagonist in morphine-dependent monkeys: Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science Vol 11(4) Oct-Dec 1976, 203-221.
  • Goldman, P. S., & Docter, R. F. (1966). Facilitation of bar pressing and "suppression" of conditioned suppression in cats as a function of alcohol: Psychopharmacologia 9(1) 1966, 64-72.
  • Gonzalez, L. P., & Altshuler, H. L. (1979). Scopolamine effects on suppression of operant responding: Physiological Psychology Vol 7(2) Jun 1979, 156-162.
  • Gonzalez Reyes, F. (1998). The phenomenon of collateral excitation in differential inhibition: Additional effects of the extinction of excitatory training cues: Psicologica Vol 19(3) 1998, 275-293.
  • Gonzalez Reyes, F. (1999). The change of the associative value of the context in differential inhibition: Effects of the extinction and the overshadowing of the contextual cues: Psicologica Vol 20(2) 1999, 103-119.
  • Gonzalo de la Casa, L., & Ruiz, G. (1990). Context specificity of latent inhibition in pigeons (Columba livia): Attenuation of the effect of delay in familiar contexts: Psicologica Vol 11(2) 1990, 177-188.
  • Good, M., & Macphail, E. M. (1994). Hippocampal lesions in pigeons (Columba livia) disrupt reinforced preexposure but not overshadowing or blocking: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 47B(3) Aug 1994, 263-291.
  • Goodall, G. (1984). Learning due to the response-shock contingency in signalled punishment: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 36B(3) Aug 1984, 259-279.
  • Goodall, G., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1987). Analysis of the Pavlovian properties of signals for punishment: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 39B(1) Feb 1987, 1-21.
  • Gordon, W. C. (1975). The use of untrained index response in a conditioned suppression paradigm: Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation Vol 7(5) Sep 1975, 452-454.
  • Gordon, W. C., McGinnis, C. M., & Weaver, M. S. (1985). The effect of cuing after backward conditioning trials: Learning and Motivation Vol 16(4) Nov 1985, 444-463.
  • Gouvier, W. D., & Yehle, A. L. (1979). Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol induced response suppression in pigeons: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 13(4) Apr 1979, 261-262.
  • Graeff, F. G., & Silveira Filho, N. G. (1978). Behavioral inhibition induced by electrical stimulation of the median raphe nucleus of the rat: Physiology & Behavior Vol 21(4) Oct 1978, 477-484.
  • Graham, F. K. (1943). Conditioned inhibition and conditioned excitation in transfer of discrimination: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 33(5) Nov 1943, 351-368.
  • Grahame, N. J., Barnet, R. C., Gunther, L. M., & Miller, R. R. (1994). Latent inhibition as a performance deficit resulting from CS-context associations: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 22(4) Nov 1994, 395-408.
  • Grahame, N. J., Barnet, R. C., & Miller, R. R. (1992). Pavlovian conditioning in multiple contexts: Competition between contexts for comparator status: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 20(4) Nov 1992, 329-338.
  • Grahame, N. J., Barnet, R. C., & Miller, R. R. (1992). Pavlovian inhibition cannot be obtained by posttraining A-US pairings: Further evidence for the empirical asymmetry of the comparator hypothesis: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 30(5) Sep 1992, 399-402.
  • Grahame, N. J., Hallam, S. C., Geier, L., & Miller, R. R. (1990). Context as an occasion setter following either CS acquisition and extinction or CS acquisition alone: Learning and Motivation Vol 21(3) Aug 1990, 237-265.
  • Grauer, E., & Thomas, E. (1982). Conditioned suppression of medial forebrain bundle and septal intracranial self-stimulation in the rat: Evidence for a fear-relief mechanism of the septum: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 96(1) Feb 1982, 61-70.
  • Graves, H. B. (1981). Detection of a 60-Hz electric field by pigeons: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 32(2) Jun 1981, 229-234.
  • Gray, J. A., Joseph, M. H., Hemsley, D. R., Young, A. M. J., Warburton, E. C., Boulenguez, P., et al. (1995). The role of mesolimbic dopaminergic and retohippocampal afferents to the nucleus accumbens in latent inhibition: Implications for schizophrenia: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 71(1-2) Nov 1995, 19-31.
  • Gray, L., & Rubel, E. W. (1981). Development of responsiveness to suprathreshold acoustic stimulation in chickens: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 95(1) Feb 1981, 188-198.
  • Gray, T. (1978). Blocking in the CER: Trace and delay procedures: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 32(1) Mar 1978, 40-42.
  • Gray, T. (1979). Sudden, rapid acquisition of conditioned suppression (CER): Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 7(3) Aug 1979, 351-354.
  • Grechenko, T. N. (1990). Conditioned inhibition of action potential generation in isolated Helix pomatia neurons: Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology Vol 20(5) Sep-Oct 1990, 452-459.
  • Greenough, T. J. (1977). An analogue study of specific parameters of overt and covert aversive conditioning: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Grelle, M. H. (1979). Role of relaxation in the conditioned inhibitory effect: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Grelle, M. J., & James, J. H. (1981). Conditioned Inhibition of Fear: Evidence for a Competing Response Mechanism: Learning and Motivation Vol 12(3) Aug 1981, 300-320.
  • Grigson, P. S., & Hajnal, A. (2007). Once is too much: Conditioned changes in accumbens dopamine following a single saccharin-morphine pairing: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 121(6) Dec 2007, 1234-1242.
  • Grigson, P. S., Twining, R. C., & Carelli, R. M. (2000). Heroin-induced suppression of saccharin intake in water-deprived and water-replete rats: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 66(3) Jul 2000, 603-608.
  • Grillon, C., & Ameli, R. (2001). Conditioned inhibition of fear-potentiated startle and skin conductance in humans: Psychophysiology Vol 38(5) Sep 2001, 807-815.
  • Grossberg, S., & Schmajuk, N. A. (1987). Neural dynamics of attentionally modulated Pavlovian conditioning: Conditioned reinforcement, inhibition, and opponent processing: Psychobiology Vol 15(3) Sep 1987, 195-240.
  • Grosslight, J. H., & Ticknor, W. (1953). Variability and reactive inhibition in the meal worm as a function of determined turning sequences: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 46(1) Feb 1953, 35-38.
  • Grove, R. N., & Schuster, C. R. (1974). Suppression of cocaine self-administration by extinction and punishment: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 2(2) Mar 1974, 199-208.
  • Grover, L. M., & Farley, J. (1987). Temporal order sensitivity of associative neural and behavioral changes in Hermissenda: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 101(5) Oct 1987, 658-675.
  • Gruber, T., Giabbiconi, C.-M., Trujillo-Barreto, N. J., & Muller, M. M. (2006). Repetition suppression of induced gamma band responses is eliminated by task switching: European Journal of Neuroscience Vol 24(9) Nov 2006, 2654-2660.
  • Grundt, A. M. (2000). Dismantling instruction to distract from a painful stimulus: Approach/avoidance functions of distracting instructions. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Gunther, L. M., Cole, R. P., & Miller, R. R. (1998). Overshadowing of occasion setting: Learning and Motivation Vol 29(3) Aug 1998, 323-344.
  • Gunther, L. M., Denniston, J. C., & Miller, R. R. (1998). Conducting exposure treatment in multiple contexts can prevent relapse: Behaviour Research and Therapy Vol 36(1) Jan 1998, 75-91.
  • Gupta, P., & Brush, F. R. (1998). Differential behavioral and endocrinological effects of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) in the Syracuse high- and low-avoidance rats: Hormones and Behavior Vol 34(3) Dec 1998, 262-267.
  • Gurnani, P. D., & Vaughan, M. (1981). Changes in frequency and distress during prolonged repetition of obsessional thoughts: British Journal of Clinical Psychology Vol 20(2) Jun 1981, 79-81.
  • Gustafson, J. W. (1972). Alterations by hippocampal afterdischarges in the control of behavior by external cues: Dissertation Abstracts International Vol.
  • Gustafson, J. W., Lidsky, T. I., & Schwartzbaum, J. S. (1975). Effects of hippocampal stimulation on acquisition, extinction, and generalization of conditioned suppression in the rat: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 89(10) Dec 1975, 1136-1148.
  • Gutman, A. (1977). Positive contrast, negative induction and inhibitory stimulus control in the rat: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Hagan, J. J. (1983). Post training lysine-vasopressin in injections increase conditioned suppression in rats: Physiology & Behavior Vol 31(6) Dec 1983, 765-769.
  • Haggbloom, S. J. (1983). Blocking and partial reinforcement: Effects of N transitions early vs. late in training on resistance to extinction: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 21(2) Mar 1983, 153-156.
  • Haggbloom, S. J. (1983). Blocking and partial reinforcement: Effects of N-R transitions early vs. late in training on resistance to extinction: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 21(2) Mar 1983, 153-156.
  • Hagopian, L. P., & Ollendick, T. H. (1996). Behavioral inhibition and anxiety sensitivity: A reanalysis: Personality and Individual Differences Vol 21(2) Aug 1996, 247-252.
  • Hake, D. F., & Campbell, R. L. (1980). Some relations between classically conditioned aggression and conditioned suppression in squirrel monkeys: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 34(2) Sep 1980, 149-165.
  • Hale, M. W., & Crowe, S. F. (2002). The effects of selective dopamine agonists on a passive avoidance learning task in the day-old chick: Behavioural Pharmacology Vol 13(4) Jul 2002, 295-301.
  • Hall, G., & Minor, H. (1984). A search for context-stimulus associations in latent inhibition: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 36B(2) May 1984, 145-169.
  • Hall, G., & Pearce, J. M. (1982). Restoring the associability of a pre-exposed CS by a surprising event: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 34B(3) Aug 1982, 127-140.
  • Hall, G., Ray, E., & Bonardi, C. (1993). Acquired equivalence between cues trained with a common antecedent: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 19(4) Oct 1993, 391-399.
  • Hall, G., & Schachtman, T. R. (1987). Differential effects of a retention interval on latent inhibition and the habituation of an orienting response: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 15(1) Feb 1987, 76-82.
  • Hall, J. F. (1984). Backward conditioning in Pavlovian type studies: Reevaluation and present status: Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science Vol 19(4) Oct-Dec 1984, 163-168.
  • Hall, J. F. (1986). The conditional emotional response as a model of Pavlovian conditioning: Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science Vol 21(1) Jan-Mar 1986, 1-11.
  • Hallam, S. C., Grahame, N. J., & Miller, R. R. (1992). Exploring the edges of Pavlovian contingency space: An assessment of contingency theory and its various metrics: Learning and Motivation Vol 23(3) Aug 1992, 225-249.
  • Hallam, S. C., Matzel, L. D., Sloat, J. S., & Miller, R. R. (1990). Excitation and inhibition as a function of posttraining extinction of the excitatory cue used in Pavlovian inhibition training: Learning and Motivation Vol 21(1) Feb 1990, 59-84.
  • Hammer, C. E. (1975). Reinforcement and suppression: The functions of the aversive stimulus: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Hammond, L. J. (1995). Pavlovian conditioning to contexts that precede the place where shock occurs as measured by freezing and activity suppression in mice: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 23(3) Aug 1995, 349-356.
  • Hammond, P., & Kim, J. N. (1996). Role of suppression in shaping orientation and direction selectivity of complex neurons in cat striate cortex: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 75(3) Mar 1996, 1163-1176.
  • Hancock, R. A., & Ayres, J. J. (1974). Within-subject effects of sucrose concentration on conditioned suppression of licking: Psychological Record Vol 24(3) Sum 1974, 325-331.
  • Hanford, P. V., Mulvaney, D. E., & Kelfer, D. A. (1980). The effect of novel environments on CS extinction in a conditioned suppression paradigm: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 16(5) Nov 1980, 341-344.
  • Hansen, P. O., Svensson, P., Arendt-Nielsen, L., & Jensen, T. S. (2002). Human masseter inhibitory reflexes evoked by repetitive electrical stimulation: Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 113(2) Feb 2002, 236-242.
  • Harder, D. M. (1977). A comparison of the effects of timeout, punishment and extinction on an aggressive behavior in children: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Harmer, C. J., & Phillips, G. D. (1999). Enhanced conditioned inhibition following repeated pretreatment with d-amphetamine: Psychopharmacology Vol 142(2) Feb 1999, 120-131.
  • Haroutunian, V., Riccio, D. C., & Gans, D. P. (1976). Suppression of drinking following rotational stimulation as an index of motion sickness in the rat: Physiological Psychology Vol 4(4) Dec 1976, 467-472.
  • Harrigan, T., Peredery, O., & Persinger, M. A. (1990). Failure to acquire an inhibitory task following seizure-induced brain damage: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 70(1) Feb 1990, 268-270.
  • Harris, G. C., & Aston-Jones, G. (2003). Altered motivation and learning following opiate withdrawal: Evidence for prolonged dysregulation of reward processing: Neuropsychopharmacology Vol 28(5) May 2003, 865-871.
  • Harris, J. B. (2005). Differential conditioning of alpha amplitude: A fresh look at an old phenomenon: Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 116(6) Jun 2005, 1433-1443.
  • Hartman, T. F., & Grant, D. A. (1962). Differential eyelid conditioning as a function of the CS-UCS interval: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 64(2) Aug 1962, 131-136.
  • Harzem, P., Lowe, C. F., & Priddle-Higson, P. J. (1978). Inhibiting function of reinforcement: Magnitude effects on variable-interval schedules: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 30(1) Jul 1978, 1-10.
  • Haug, T., & Gotestam, K. G. (1981). Two opposite effects of diazepam on fear by differential training in the CER-paradigm: Psychopharmacology Vol 75(2) Nov 1981, 110-113.
  • Heil, M., Osman, A., Wiegelmann, J., Rolke, B., & Hennighausen, E. (2000). N200 in the Eriksen-task: Inhibitory executive process? : Journal of Psychophysiology Vol 14(4) 2000, 218-225.
  • Heldt, S. A., & Falls, W. A. (1998). Destruction of the auditory thalamus disrupts the production of fear but not the inhibition of fear conditioned to an auditory stimulus: Brain Research Vol 813(2) Dec 1998, 274-282.
  • Hellemans, K. G. C., Everitt, B. J., & Lee, J. L. C. (2006). Disrupting reconsolidation of conditioned withdrawal memories in the basolateral amygdala reduces suppression of heroin seeking in rats: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 26(49) Dec 2006, 12694-12699.
  • Hellstern, F., Malaka, R., & Hammer, M. (1998). Backward inhibitory learning in honeybees: A behaviorial analysis of reinforcement processing: Learning & Memory Vol 4(5) Jan-Feb 1998, 429-444.
  • Hendersen, R. W. (1975). Compounds of conditioned fear stimuli: Learning and Motivation Vol 6(1) Feb 1975, 28-42.
  • Hendersen, R. W. (1978). Forgetting of conditioned fear inhibition: Learning and Motivation Vol 9(1) Feb 1978, 16-30.
  • Hendricks, J. (1966). Flicker thresholds as determined by a modified conditioned suppression procedure: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 9(5) 1966, 501-506.
  • Hendry, D. P., & Van-Toller, C. (1965). Alleviation of conditioned suppression: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 59(3) Jun 1965, 458-460.
  • Hendry, J. S. (1982). Summation of undetected excitation following extinction of the CER: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 10(4) Nov 1982, 476-482.
  • Hennevin, E., & Hars, B. (1992). Second-order conditioning during sleep: Psychobiology Vol 20(2) Jun 1992, 166-176.
  • Hernandez, L. L., & Valentine, J. D. (1989). Enhancement of Pavlovian conditioned suppression by mild ethanol intoxication: Psychopharmacology Vol 97(4) Apr 1989, 476-480.
  • Herring, B. (1979). Comments on paper by Dunham: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 7(2) May 1979, 271-272.
  • Heth, C. D. (1975). Simultaneous and backward Pavlovian conditioning as a function on number of CS-US pairings: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Hikichi, T., Akiyoshi, J., Yamamoto, Y., Tsutsumi, T., Isogawa, K., & Nagayama, H. (2000). Suppression of conditioned fear by administration of CRF receptor antagonist CP-154,526: Pharmacopsychiatry Vol 33(5) Sep 2000, 189-193.
  • Hilgard, E. R. (1937). The relationship between the conditioned response and conventional learning experiments: Psychological Bulletin Vol 34(2) Feb 1937, 61-102.
  • Himle, M. B., Woods, D. W., Conelea, C. A., Bauer, C. C., & Rice, K. A. (2007). Investigating the effects of tic suppression on premonitory urge ratings in children and adolescents with Tourette's syndrome: Behaviour Research and Therapy Vol 45(12) Dec 2007, 2964-2976.
  • Hingtgen, J. N., Shekhar, A., DiMicco, J. A., & Aprison, M. H. (1988). Response suppression in rats after bilateral microinjection of 5-hydroxytryptophan in lateral hypothalamus: Biological Psychiatry Vol 23(7) Apr 1988, 711-718.
  • Hinson, R. E., & Siegel, S. (1986). Pavlovian inhibitory conditioning and tolerance to pentobarbital-induced hypothermia in rats: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 12(4) Oct 1986, 363-370.
  • Hirano, T. (1980). A comparison of the familiarity effect of electroconvulsive shock and hippocampal afterdischarges on conditioned suppression: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 22(2) Jul 1980, 82-89.
  • Hittesdorf, W. M., & Richards, R. W. (1978). Aversive second-order conditioning in the pigeon: Psychological Record Vol 28(4) Fal 1978, 605-613.
  • Hoehn-Saric, R., McLeod, D. R., & Glowa, J. R. (1991). The effects of NMDA receptor blockade on the acquisition of a conditioned emotional response: Biological Psychiatry Vol 30(2) Jul 1991, 170-176.
  • Hoffman, H. S., Flesher, M., & Jensen, P. (1963). Stimulus aspects of aversive controls: The retention of conditioned suppression: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 6(4) 1963, 575-583.
  • Hoffman, H. S., & Fleshler, M. (1962). The course of emotionality in the development of avoidance: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 64(3) Sep 1962, 288-294.
  • Hoffman, H. S., & Fleshler, M. (1964). Stimulus aspects of aversive controls: Stimulus generalization of conditioned suppression following discrimination training: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 7(3) 1964, 233-239.
  • Holland, P. C. (1979). Differential effects of omission contingencies on various components of Pavlovian appetitive conditioned responding in rats: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 5(2) Apr 1979, 178-193.
  • Holland, P. C. (1984). Differential effects of reinforcement of an inhibitory feature after serial and simultaneous feature negative discrimination training: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 10(4) Oct 1984, 461-475.
  • Holland, P. C. (1984). Unblocking in Pavlovian appetitive conditioning: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 10(4) Oct 1984, 476-497.
  • Holland, P. C. (1988). Excitation and inhibition in unblocking: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 14(3) Jul 1988, 261-279.
  • Holland, P. C. (1989). Transfer of negative occasion setting and conditioned inhibition across conditioned and unconditioned stimuli: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 15(4) Oct 1989, 311-328.
  • Holland, P. C., & Gory, J. (1986). Extinction of inhibition after serial and simultaneous feature negative discrimination training: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 38(3-B) Aug 1986, 245-265.
  • Holland, P. C., & Lamarre, J. (1984). Transfer of inhibition after serial and simultaneous feature negative discriminative training: Learning and Motivation Vol 15(3) Aug 1984, 219-243.
  • Holt, L., & Gray, J. A. (1985). Proactive behavioral effects of theta-driving septal stimulation on conditioned suppression and punishment in the rat: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 99(1) Feb 1985, 60-74.
  • Houpt, T. A., Cassell, J. A., Riccardi, C., Kwon, B., & Smith, J. C. (2007). Suppression of drinking by exposure to a high-strength static magnetic field: Physiology & Behavior Vol 90(1) Jan 2007, 59-65.
  • Houser, V. P., & Houser, F. L. (1973). The effects of agents that modify muscarinic tone upon behavior controlled by an avoidance schedule that employs signaled unavoidable shock: Psychopharmacologia Vol 32(2) 1973, 133-150.
  • Hoyert, M. S., & Zeiler, M. D. (1995). Stimulus properties of the differential-reinforcement-of-not-responding schedule: Psychological Record Vol 45(1) Win 1995, 73-93.
  • Huang, A. C. W., & Hsiao, S. (2002). Haloperidol attenuates rewarding and aversively conditioned suppression of saccharin solution intake: Reevaluation of the anhedonia hypothesis of dopamine blocking: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 116(4) Aug 2002, 646-650.
  • Hughes, H. H., Holder, B., & Dubois, K. E. (1978). One session of flooding as treatment for conditioned avoidance responding in humans: Effect of individualization of treatment duration: Psychological Reports Vol 42(2) Apr 1978, 663-672.
  • Hunt, T., Poulos, C. X., & Cappell, H. (1990). A contingent, conditioned suppression of eating following chronic benzodiazepine-induced hyperphagia: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 35(2) Feb 1990, 373-378.
  • Huppert, F. A., & Iversen, S. D. (1975). Response suppression in rats: A comparison of response-contingent and noncontingent punishment and the effect of the minor tranquilizer, chlordiazepoxide: Psychopharmacologia Vol 44(1) 1975, 67-75.
  • Hurwitz, H. M. (1974). A note on the conditioned stimulus control of postshock responding: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 4(6) Dec 1974, 554-556.
  • Hurwitz, H. M., & Davis, H. (1983). The description and analysis of conditioned suppression: A critique of the conventional suppression ratio: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 11(4) Nov 1983, 383-390.
  • Hutchinson, R. R., & Emley, G. S. (1973). Effects of nicotine on avoidance, conditioned suppression and aggression response measures in animal and man. Oxford, England: V H Winston & Sons.
  • Hyde, T. S. (1976). The effects of Pavlovian CSs on two food-reinforced baselines--with and without noncontingent shock: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 4(3) Aug 1976, 293-298.
  • Hymowitz, N. (1973). Comparisons between variable-interval and fixed-interval schedules of electric shock delivery: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 19(1) Jan 1973, 101-111.
  • Hymowitz, N. (1976). Effects on responding of mixed and multiple schedules of signalled and unsignalled response-dependent electric-shock delivery: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 25(3) May 1976, 321-326.
  • Hymowitz, N. (1977). Effects of the intensity of electric shock on response suppression during multiple and mixed schedules of signalled and unsignalled electric-shock delivery: Psychological Record Vol 27(2) Spr 1977, 425-440.
  • Hymowitz, N. (1979). Suppression of responding during signaled and unsignaled shock: Psychological Bulletin Vol 86(1) Jan 1979, 175-190.
  • Hymowitz, N. (1981). Effects of diazepam on schedule-controlled and schedule-induced behavior under signaled and unsignaled shock: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 36(1) Jul 1981, 119-132.
  • Hymowitz, N. (1981). Effects of signaled and unsignaled shock on schedule-controlled lever pressing and schedule-induced licking: Shock intensity and body weight: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 35(2) Mar 1981, 197-207.
  • Iida, N., & Kimura, H. (2007). Effects of variable-interval punishment on lever pressing maintained by variable-ratio reinforcement in the rat: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 78(5) Dec 2007, 528-533.
  • Iles, J. F., & Pardoe, J. (1999). Changes in transmission in the pathway of heteronymous spinal recurrent inhibition from soleus to quadriceps motor neurons during movement in man: Brain: A Journal of Neurology Vol 122(9) Sep 1999, 1757-1764.
  • Illich, P. A., Joynes, R. L., & Walters, E. T. (1994). Response-specific inhibition during general facilitation of defensive responses in Aplysia: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 108(3) Jun 1994, 614-623.
  • Illich, P. A., Salinas, J. A., & Grau, J. W. (1994). Latent inhibition, overshadowing, and blocking of a conditioned antinociceptive response in spinalized rats: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 62(2) Sep 1994, 140-150.
  • Imada, H., & et al. (1978). The effects of double-alternation schedules of shock intensity upon patterning of suppression of licking in rats with special reference to cue-utilization: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 20(4) Dec 1978, 167-176.
  • Imada, H., & Kitaguchi, K. (2002). Recent learned helplessness/irrelevance research in Japan: Conceptual framework and some experiments in learned irrelevance: Integrative Physiological & Behavioral Science Vol 37(1) Jan-Mar 2002, 9-21.
  • Imada, H., Shuku, H., & Moriya, M. (1983). Can a rat count? : Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 11(4) Nov 1983, 396-400.
  • Imada, H., Yamazaki, A., & Morishita, M. (1981). The effects of signal intensity upon conditioned suppression: Effects upon responding during signals and intersignal intervals: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 9(2) May 1981, 269-274.
  • Imada, S., Kondo, H., & Imada, H. (1985). Effects of shocks, presented at a fixed time of day, on appetitive and general activity of rats: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 13(2) May 1985, 194-200.
  • Ishii, K. (1986). Stimulus selection in the rat: Comparison of "blocking" and "overshadowing" effects in compound conditioning: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 57(5) Dec 1986, 300-306.
  • Ishii, K. (1990). Effects of the durations of conditioned stimulus and non-CS period on conditioned suppression in the rat: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 32(2) 1990, 97-100.
  • Ishii, K. (1999). Attenuation of latent inhibition after compound conditioning: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 41(2) May 1999, 102-111.
  • Ison, J. R., & Ash, B. (1977). Effects of experience on stimulus-produced reflex inhibition in the human: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 10(6) Dec 1977, 467-468.
  • Ison, J. R., Zuckerman, M., & Russo, J. M. (1975). Combination rules for inhibitory stimuli: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 1(4) Oct 1975, 318-325.
  • Izawa, Y., Suzuki, H., & Shinoda, Y. (2004). Suppression of Visually and Memory-Guided Saccades Induced by Electrical Stimulation of the Monkey Frontal Eye Field. II. Suppression of Bilateral Saccades: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 92(4) Oct 2004, 2261-2273.
  • Jackson, D. E. (1974). CS-free food contingencies and subsequent acquisition of conditioned suppression: No transfer effect: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 4(4-A) Oct 1974, 235-236.
  • Jackson, D. E., & Delprato, D. J. (1974). Aversive CSs suppress lever pressing for food but not the eating of free food: Learning and Motivation Vol 5(4) Nov 1974, 448-458.
  • Jackson, M. F., Dennis, T., Esplin, B., & Capek, R. (1994). Acute effects of !g-vinyl GABA (vigabatrin) on hippocampal GABAergic inhibition in vitro: Brain Research Vol 651(1-2) Jul 1994, 85-91.
  • Jackson, R. L., Maier, S. F., & Rapaport, P. M. (1978). Exposure to inescapable shock produces both activity and associative deficits in the rat: Learning and Motivation Vol 9(1) Feb 1978, 69-98.
  • Jacobs, W. J., & Blackburn, J. R. (1987). Incubation of conditional suppression is an associatively-based phenomenon: Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science Vol 22(3) Jul-Sep 1987, 118-121.
  • James, J. H., & Wanger, A. R. (1980). One-trial overshadowing: Evidence of distributive processing: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 6(2) Apr 1980, 188-205.
  • James, J. P., Shangi, L., Pangman, C., & Mostoway, W. (1973). The role of number and spacing of CS-US pairings in conditioned suppression: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 1(2) May 1973, 140-144.
  • Janssen, J. (2002). Fantasy becomes reality. Treatment of sexual fantasies of sex-offenders: Tijdschrift voor Psychotherapie Vol 28(3) May 2002, 223-246.
  • Janssen, M., Farley, J., & Hearst, E. (1995). Temporal location of unsignaled food deliveries: Effects on conditioned withdrawal (inhibition) in pigeon signtracking: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 21(2) Apr 1995, 116-128.
  • Jastreboff, P. J., Brennan, J. F., Coleman, J. K., & Sasaki, C. T. (1988). Phantom auditory sensation in rats: An animal model for tinnitus: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 102(6) Dec 1988, 811-822.
  • Javier Vila, C., & Florencio Miranda, H. (1994). Pavlovian inhibitory conditioning and tolerance-reversal to scopolamine-induced adipsia in rats: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 61(2) Mar 1994, 177-180.
  • Jessen, F., Kucharski, C., Fries, T., Papassotiropoulos, A., Hoenig, K., Maier, W., et al. (2001). Sensory gating deficit expressed by a disturbed suppression of the P50 event-related potential in patients with Alzheimer's disease: American Journal of Psychiatry Vol 158(8) Aug 2001, 1319-1321.
  • Jie, L., Wenjuan, L., Li, Z., & Bo, L. (2004). Conditioned immunosuppression and the expression of C-FOS in the hypothalamic nuclei: Acta Psychologica Sinica Vol 36(2) Mar 2004, 201-207.
  • Jin, Z., & Zhang, Y. (1995). A study on negative priming effect and distractor identity inhibition in categorization task: Acta Psychologica Sinica Vol 27(4) 1995, 344-349.
  • Johansson, C., Jackson, D. M., & Svensson, L. (1994). The atypical antipsychotic, remoxipride, blocks phencyclidine-induced disruption of prepulse inhibition in the rat: Psychopharmacology Vol 116(4) Dec 1994, 437-442.
  • Jones, D., & Gonzalez-Lima, F. (2001). Associative effects of Pavlovian differential inhibition of behaviour: European Journal of Neuroscience Vol 14(11) Dec 2001, 1915-1927.
  • Jordan, W. P., & Leaton, R. N. (1982). Effects of mesencephalic reticular formation lesions on habituation of startle and lick suppression responses in the rat: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 96(2) Apr 1982, 170-183.
  • Jordan, W. P., Strasser, H. C., & McHale, L. (2000). Contextual control of long-term habituation in rats: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 26(3) Jul 2000, 323-339.
  • Josselyn, S. A., Falls, W. A., Gewirtz, J. C., Pistell, P., & Davis, M. (2005). The Nucleus Accumbens is not Critically Involved in Mediating the Effects of a Safety Signal on Behavior: Neuropsychopharmacology Vol 30(1) Jan 2005, 17-26.
  • Kadden, R. M. (1973). Facilitation and suppression of responding under temporally defined schedules of negative reinforcement: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 19(3) May 1973, 469-480.
  • Kadden, R. M. (1974). Stimulus distribution and response-dependence as parameters of temporally defined schedules of negative reinforcement: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Kameyama, T., Nabeshima, T., & Yamada, K. (1985). Differences of alteration in opioid systems induced by conditioned suppression and electric footshock in mice: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 22(2) Feb 1985, 249-254.
  • Kameyama, T., & Nagasaka, M. (1982). The effects of analgesics on quickly-learned conditioned suppression in mice: Neuropharmacology Vol 21(12) Dec 1982, 1283-1289.
  • Kameyama, T., & Nagasaka, M. (1983). Effect of naloxone on a quickly learned conditioned suppression in rats following cessation of chronic haloperidol treatment: Brain Research Vol 275(2) Sep 1983, 389-391.
  • Kameyama, T., & Nagasaka, M. (1983). Effects of apomorphine and methamphetamine on a quickly-learned conditioned-suppression response in rats: Neuropharmacology Vol 22(7) Jul 1983, 813-817.
  • Kameyama, T., Nagasaka, M., & Yamada, K. (1985). Effects of antidepressant drugs on a quickly-learned conditioned-suppression response in mice: Neuropharmacology Vol 24(4) Apr 1985, 285-290.
  • Kamin, L. J., Brimer, C. J., & Black, A. H. (1963). Conditioned suppression as a monitor of fear of the CS in the course of avoidance training: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 56(3) Jun 1963, 497-501.
  • Kamin, L. J., & Idrobo, F. (1978). Configural conditioning in the CER: A possible artifact: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 6(3) Aug 1978, 290-293.
  • Kamin, L. J., & Szakmary, G. A. (1977). Configural-like effects observed in the course of second-order conditioning in rats: Learning and Motivation Vol 8(1) Feb 1977, 126-135.
  • Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., Overstreet, D. H., Rezvani, A. H., & Janowsky, D. S. (1995). Suppression of ethanol intake in alcohol-preferring rats by prior voluntary saccharin consumption: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 52(1) Sep 1995, 59-64.
  • Kaneshige, K., Nakajima, S., & Imada, H. (2001). The effect of on- or off-line extinction of a first-order conditioned stimulus on a second-order conditioned response in rats: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 43(2) May 2001, 91-97.
  • Kaplan, P. S. (1984). Importance of relative temporal parameters in trace autoshaping: From excitation to inhibition: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 10(2) Apr 1984, 113-126.
  • Karazinov, D. M., & Boakes, R. A. (2004). Learning about cues that prevent an outcome: Conditioned inhibition and differential inhibition in human predictive learning: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 57B(2) Apr 2004, 153-178.
  • Karpicke, J. (1978). Directed approach responses and positive conditioned suppression in the rat: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 6(2) May 1978, 216-224.
  • Karpicke, J., Christoph, G., Peterson, G., & Hearst, E. (1977). Signal location and positive versus negative conditioned suppression in the rat: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 3(2) Apr 1977, 105-118.
  • Karpicke, J., & Dout, D. (1980). Withdrawal from signals for imminent inescapable electric shock: Psychological Record Vol 30(4) Fal 1980, 511-523.
  • Karpicke, J. A. (1977). A competing-response analysis of positive conditioned suppression: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Kasprow, W. J., Catterson, D., Schachtman, T. R., & Miller, R. R. (1984). Attenuation of latent inhibition by post-acquisition reminder: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 36B(1) Feb 1984, 53-63.
  • Kasprow, W. J., Schachtman, T. R., & Miller, R. R. (1987). The comparator hypothesis of conditioned response generation: Manifest conditioned excitation and inhibition as a function of relative excitatory strengths of CS and conditioning context at the time of testing: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 13(4) Oct 1987, 395-406.
  • Katoh, A., Nabeshima, T., Kuno, A., Wada, M., & et al. (1996). Changes in striatal dopamine release in stress-induced conditioned suppression of motility in rats: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 77(1-2) May 1996, 219-221.
  • Kaufman, A. (1964). Increased suppression during punishment applied to the responding member: Psychonomic Science Vol 1(10) 1964, 310-311.
  • Kawai, N. (1996). Differential conditioning of UCS duration in rats: The effects of the intertransitional interval: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 67(5) Dec 1996, 396-402.
  • Kawai, N. (2000). The number of trials upon the effects of US duration in conditioned licking suppression with rats: Japanese Journal of Animal Psychology Vol 50(1) Jun 2000, 21-26.
  • Kawai, N., & Imada, H. (1990). Attentuation of conditioned suppression by immediately preceding primary and secondary aversive stimuli in rats: Japanese Journal of Animal Psychology Vol 40(2) 1990, 20-28.
  • Kawai, N., & Imada, H. (1996). Between- and within-subject effects of US duration on conditioned suppression in rats: Contrast makes otherwise unnoticed duration dimension stand out: Learning and Motivation Vol 27(1) Feb 1996, 92-111.
  • Kawai, N., Nishida, N., & Imada, H. (1998). Effects of postconditioning manipulations following compound conditioning on conditioned licking suppression in rats: Psychologia: An International Journal of Psychology in the Orient Vol 41(1) Mar 1998, 49-59.
  • Kaye, H., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1990). A change of context can enhance performance of an aversive but not of an appetitive conditioned response: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 42(2-B) May 1990, 113-134.
  • Kazdin, A. E. (1972). Response cost: The removal of conditioned reinforcers for therapeutic change: Behavior Therapy Vol 3(4) Oct 1972, 533-546.
  • Kearns, D. N., Weiss, S. J., & Panlilio, L. V. (2002). Conditioned suppression of behavior maintained by cocaine self-administration: Drug and Alcohol Dependence Vol 65(3) Feb 2002, 253-261.
  • Kearns, D. N., Weiss, S. J., Schindler, C. W., & Panlilio, L. V. (2005). Conditioned Inhibition of Cocaine Seeking in Rats: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 31(2) Apr 2005, 247-253.
  • Kelley, D. D. (1973). Suppression of random-ratio and acceleration of temporally spaced responding by the same prereward stimulus in monkeys: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 20(3) Nov 1973, 363-373.
  • Kelly, D. D. (1973). Long-term prereward suppression in monkeys unaccompanied by cardiovascular conditioning: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 20(1) Jul 1973, 93-104.
  • Kelly, D. D. (1980). Enhancement of conditioned autonomic responses in monkeys when preshock signals occasion operant suppression: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 33(2) Mar 1980, 275-284.
  • Kelly, E. L. (1934). An experimental attempt to produce artificial chromaesthesia by the technique of the conditioned response: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 17(3) Jun 1934, 315-341.
  • Kelly, J. B., & Glazier, S. J. (1978). Auditory cortex lesions and discrimination of spatial location by the rat: Brain Research Vol 145(2) Apr 1978, 315-321.
  • Kelly, J. B., & Masterton, B. (1977). Auditory sensitivity of the albino rat: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 91(4) Aug 1977, 930-936.
  • Kelly, S. P., Lalor, E. C., Reilly, R. B., & Foxe, J. J. (2006). Increases in Alpha Oscillatory Power Reflect an Active Retinotopic Mechanism for Distracter Suppression During Sustained Visuospatial Attention: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 95(6) Jun 2006, 3844-3851.
  • Kendrick, D. C. (1958). Inhibition with reinforcement (conditioned inhibition): Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 56(4) Oct 1958, 313-318.
  • Kierniesky, N., Beaton, R., McAbee, D., & Sheftic, J. (1980). Acquisition of an avoidance response in rats during conditioned suppression: Journal of General Psychology Vol 103(2) Oct 1980, 221-225.
  • Kilbourne, B., & Fox, R. A. (1976). Discriminative control and response maintenance by a brief aversive stimulus in a fixed-interval schedule: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 8(6) Dec 1976, 453-456.
  • Killcross, A. S., Dickinson, A., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Amphetamine-induced disruption of latent inhibition are reinforcer mediated: Implications for animal models of schizophrenia attentional dysfunction: Psychopharmacology Vol 115(1-2) Jun 1994, 185-195.
  • Killcross, A. S., Dickinson, A., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Effects of the neuroleptic !a-flupenthixol on latent inhibition in aversively- and appetitively-motivated paradigms: Evidence for dopamine-reinforcer interactions: Psychopharmacology Vol 115(1-2) Jun 1994, 196-205.
  • Killcross, A. S., & Robbins, T. W. (1993). Differential effects of intra-accumbens and systemic amphetamine on latent inhibition using an on-baseline, within-subject conditioned suppression paradigm: Psychopharmacology Vol 110(4) Mar 1993, 479-489.
  • Killcross, S., & Balleine, B. (1996). Role of primary motivation in stimulus preexposure effects: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 22(1) Jan 1996, 32-42.
  • Killcross, S., & Dickinson, A. (1996). Contextual control of latent inhibition by the reinforcer: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 49B(1) Feb 1996, 45-59.
  • Kilts, C. D., Commissaris, R. L., & Rech, R. H. (1981). Comparison of anti-conflict drug effects in three experimental animal models of anxiety: Psychopharmacology Vol 74(3) Jul 1981, 290-296.
  • Kim, J. J., Krupa, D. J., & Thompson, R. F. (1998). Inhibitory cerebello-olivary projections and blocking effect in classical conditioning: Science Vol 279(5350) Jan 1998, 570-573.
  • Kim, S. D., Rivers, S., Bevins, R. A., & Ayres, J. J. B. (1996). Conditioned stimulus determinants of conditioned response form in Pavlovian fear conditioning: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 22(1) Jan 1996, 87-104.
  • Kimble, G. A., & Ost, J. W. P. (1961). A conditioned inhibitory process in eyelid conditioning: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 61(2) Feb 1961, 150-156.
  • Kimble, G. A., & Shatel, R. B. (1952). The relationship between two kinds of inhibition and the amount of practice: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 44(5) Nov 1952, 355-359.
  • Kitaguchi, K. (2000). Initial excitatory conditioning with the truly random control procedure in rats: The effects of density of the conditioned stimulus: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 42(3) Sep 2000, 135-143.
  • Kitaguchi, K., & Imada, H. (1995). Effects of negative contingency upon conditioned suppression of licking in rats: Systematic manipulations of session length and number of shocks: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 37(4) 1995, 210-220.
  • Klare, W. F. (1974). Activity level and secondary motivation: Fear: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 3(3-A) Mar 1974, 171-172.
  • Kleiman, M. C., & Fowler, H. (1984). Variations in explicitly unpaired training are differentially effective in producing conditioned inhibition: Learning and Motivation Vol 15(2) May 1984, 127-155.
  • Kleinman, J. S. (1979). The treatment of cockroach phobia using external inhibition: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Kling, J. O. (1972). Interstimulus intervals in conditioned suppression: Journal of General Psychology Vol 87(2) Oct 1972, 295-299.
  • Kling, J. O., & Kling, K. (1973). Spontaneous recovery of conditioned suppression: Journal of General Psychology Vol 88(1) Jan 1973, 151-152.
  • Klunder, C. S., & O'Boyle, M. (1979). Suppression of predatory behaviors in laboratory mice following lithium chloride injections or electric shock: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 7(1) Feb 1979, 13-16.
  • Knight, J. E. (2001). Functional brain mechanisms of visual selective attention in developmental dyslexia. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Koch, M., & Friauf, E. (1995). Glycine receptors in the caudal pontine reticular formation: Are they important for the inhibition of the acoustic startle response? : Brain Research Vol 671(1) Feb 1995, 63-72.
  • Kodsi, M. H., & Swerdlow, N. R. (1994). Quinolinic acid lesions of the ventral striatum reduce sensorimotor gating of acoustic startle in rats: Brain Research Vol 643(1-2) Apr 1994, 59-65.
  • Kodsi, M. H., & Swerdlow, N. R. (1995). Ventral pallidal GABA-A receptors regulate prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle: Brain Research Vol 684(1) Jun 1995, 26-35.
  • Koegel, R. L., & Covert, A. (1972). The relationship of self-stimulation to learning in autistic children: Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis Vol 5(4) Win 1972, 381-387.
  • Koetzner, L., Sumpter, T. L., & Woods, J. H. (2002). Tetrabenazine fails to antagonize a behavioral effect of cocaine in rhesus monkeys: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 71(1-2) Jan/Feb 2002, 1-5.
  • Kohler, E. A., & Ayres, J. J. (1982). Blocking with serial and simultaneous compounds in a trace conditioning procedure: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 10(3) Aug 1982, 277-287.
  • Kohler, E. F. (1979). Serial compounds and trace conditioning procedures in the Kamin blocking paradigm: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Kohut, S. J., Handler, S. L., Hertzbach, R. L., & Riley, A. L. (2007). Schedule-induced polydipsia alters cocaine-but not ethanol-induced suppression of saccharin consumption: Drug and Alcohol Dependence Vol 91(1) Nov 2007, 18-25.
  • Komiskey, H. L., Buck, M. A., Mundinger, K. L., McSweeney, F. K., & et al. (1987). Effect of aging on anticonflict and CNS depressant activity of diazepam in rats: Psychopharmacology Vol 93(4) Dec 1987, 443-448.
  • Koranda, L. G., & Freedman, P. E. (1976). Effects of generalization of fear on incubation in a response suppression paradigm: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 8(2) Aug 1976, 97-100.
  • Korner, I. N. (1966). The mechanics of suppression: An experimental investigation: Journal of Consulting Psychology Vol 30(3) Jun 1966, 269-272.
  • Kostyla, S. J. (1980). Facilitation and suppression of punished escape behavior: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Kozlova, L. N. (1964). Effects of age on the excitability of the cerebral cortex and on the equilibration of nervous processes in dogs: Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel'nosti 14(4) 1964, 678-686.
  • Kremer, M. E., Napierala, J. S., & Haude, R. H. (1978). Suppression of visual observing by rhesus monkeys produced by conditioned aversive visual stimuli: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 46(2) Apr 1978, 467-475.
  • Krestel, D., Passe, D., Smith, J. C., & Jonsson, L. (1984). Behavioral determination of olfactory thresholds to amyl acetate in dogs: Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews Vol 8(2) Sum 1984, 169-174.
  • Krishnan-Sarin, S., Wand, G. S., Li, X. W., Portoghese, P. S., & Froehlich, J. C. (1998). Effect of mu opioid receptor blockade on alcohol intake in rats bred for high alcohol drinking: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 59(3) Mar 1998, 627-635.
  • Kruchenko, Z. A., & Sachenko, V. V. (1987). Activity of neurones of the cerebral cortex of cats in conditioned inhibition: Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel'nosti Vol 37(5) Sep-Oct 1987, 906-913.
  • Kruchenko, Z. A., & Sachenko, V. V. (1988). Cortical unit activity in cats during conditioned inhibition: Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology Vol 18(5) Sep-Oct 1988, 419-425.
  • Krushinskaia, N. L. (1963). Changes in motor food CRs in pigeons following a lesion in the paleocortex: Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel'nosti 13(6) 1963, 1077-1086.
  • Kulig, J. W., & Tighe, T. J. (1981). Habituation in children within a behavior suppression paradigm: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology Vol 32(3) Dec 1981, 425-442.
  • Kuribara, H., & Tadokoro, S. (1981). Enhancement of the avoidance-suppressing effects of neuroleptic agents after repeated administration: Changes in the discriminated avoidance response in rats: Psychopharmacology Vol 75(2) Nov 1981, 148-151.
  • Kuwajima, M., & Sawaguchi, T. (2007). Involvement of the lateral prefrontal cortex in conditional suppression of gaze shift: Neuroscience Research Vol 59(4) Dec 2007, 431-445.
  • LaBarbera, J. D., & Caul, W. F. (1976). Decrement in distress to an aversive event during a conditioned positive opponent-process: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 4(4) Nov 1976, 485-489.
  • Ladenheim, E. E., Wirth, K. E., & Moran, T. H. (1996). Receptor subtype mediation of feeding suppression by bombesin-like peptides: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 54(4) Aug 1996, 705-711.
  • Lado-Abeal, J., Clapper, J. A., Zhu, B. C., Hough, C. M., Syapin, P. J., & Norman, R. L. (2002). Hypoglycemia-induced suppression of luteinizing hormone (LH) secretion in intact female rhesus macaques: Role of vasopressin and endogenous opioids: Stress: The International Journal on the Biology of Stress Vol 5(2) 2002, 113-119.
  • Lamarre, J., & Holland, P. C. (1985). Acquisition and transfer of feature-negative discriminations: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 23(1) Jan 1985, 71-74.
  • Lamarre, J., & Holland, P. C. (1987). Transfer of inhibition after serial feature negative discrimination training: Learning and Motivation Vol 18(4) Nov 1987, 319-342.
  • Lambert, J. V., Barber, R. M., Carpenito, P., Cianfrani, M., & et al. (1989). Conditioned inhibition of rotation-induced taste aversion: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 17(4) Nov 1989, 457-467.
  • Landeira-Fernandez, J., Fanselow, M. S., DeCola, J. P., & Kim, J. J. (1993). Naltrexone does not disrupt acquisition or performance of inhibitory conditioning: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 31(6) Nov 1993, 591-594.
  • Langley, W. (1981). Failure of food-aversion conditioning to suppress predatory attack of the grasshopper mouse, Onychomys leucogaster: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 33(3) Nov 1981, 317-333.
  • Langley, W. M., & Knapp, K. (1982). Importance of olfaction to suppression of attack response through conditioned taste aversion in the grasshopper mouse: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 36(4) Dec 1982, 368-378.
  • Lautenbacher, S., Roscher, S., & Strian, F. (2002). Inhibitory effects do not depend on the subjective experience of pain during heterotopic noxious conditioning stimulation (HNCS): A contribution to the psychophysics of pain inhibition: European Journal of Pain Vol 6(5) Oct 2002, 365-374.
  • Laws, D. R. (2001). Olfactory aversion: Notes on procedure, with speculations on its mechanism of effect: Sexual Abuse: Journal of Research and Treatment Vol 13(4) Oct 2001, 275-287.
  • Leaf, R. C. (1966). Motivational properties of the CS in conditioned suppression: Psychonomic Science Vol 5(1) 1966, 43-44.
  • Leaf, R. C., & Leaf, S. R. (1966). Recovery time as a measure of degree of conditioned suppression: Psychological Reports 18(1) 1966, 265-266.
  • Leaf, R. C., & Muller, S. A. (1964). Effect of CER on DRL responding: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 7(6) 1964, 405-407.
  • Leander, J. D. (1986). Peripheral action of serotonin as a model of depression: Biological Psychiatry Vol 21(8-9) Jul 1986, 842-844.
  • Leaton, R. N. (1974). Long-term retention of the habituation of lick suppression in rats: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 87(6) Dec 1974, 1157-1164.
  • Leaton, R. N. (1976). Long-term retention of the habituation of lick suppression and startle response produced by a single auditory stimulus: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 2(3) Jul 1976, 248-259.
  • Leaton, R. N., & Borszcz, G. S. (1990). Hippocampal lesions and temporally chained conditioned stimuli in a conditioned suppression paradigm: Psychobiology Vol 18(1) Mar 1990, 81-88.
  • Leclerc, R. (1985). Sign-tracking behavior in aversive conditioning: Its acquisition via a Pavlovian mechanism and its suppression by operant contingencies: Learning and Motivation Vol 16(1) Feb 1985, 63-82.
  • Lee, J. L. C., Dickinson, A., & Everitt, B. J. (2005). Conditioned suppression and freezing as measures of aversive Pavlovian conditioning: Effects of discrete amygdala lesions and overtraining: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 159(2) Apr 2005, 221-233.
  • Leitenberg, H. (1966). Conditioned acceleration and conditioned suppression in pigeons: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 9(3) 1966, 205-212.
  • Lemtiri-Chlieh, F., & Levine, E. S. (2007). Lack of depolarization-induced suppression of inhibition (DSI) in layer 2/3 interneurons that receive cannabinoid-sensitive inhibitory inputs: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 98(5) Nov 2007, 2517-2524.
  • Leonard, S., & Hall, G. (1999). Representation-mediated inhibitory learning in the conditioned-suppression procedure: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 52B(2) May 1999, 145-158.
  • LeSage, M. G., Keyler, D. E., Shoeman, D., Raphael, D., Collins, G., & Pentel, P. R. (2002). Continuous nicotine infusion reduces nicotine self-administration in rats with 23-h/day access to nicotine: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 72(1-2) May 2002, 279-289.
  • Leslie, J. C. (1977). Effects of food deprivation and reinforcement magnitude on conditioned suppression: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 28(2) Sep 1977, 107-115.
  • Leslie, J. C., & Garrud, P. (1976). Conditioned suppression of a positively reinforced shuttle response: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 4(1-A) Feb 1976, 99-104.
  • Leslie, J. C., Toal, L., & Shephard, R. A. (1995). Conditioned suppression and the effects of pentobarbital with picrotoxin, flumazenil, and RO5-3663: Psychological Record Vol 45(2) Spr 1995, 299-307.
  • Levinson, M. (1980). The effects of chlordiazepoxide and d-amphetamine on conditioned enhancement and positive conditioned suppression: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Levinson, M., & Meltzer, D. (1977). Positive conditioned suppression as a function of the percentage of CS-US pairings: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 10(2) Aug 1977, 142-144.
  • Levitan, L. J. (1975). Can a stimulus which is consistently paired with shock suppress avoidance responding? : Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Lewis, J. (1973). Conditioned suppression of a VI baseline using a two-bar multiple VI shock-avoidance schedule: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 1(4) Nov 1973, 247-250.
  • Lewis, R. A. (1976). Effects of prior shock exposure on airblast conditioned suppression in rats: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Lichtblau, L., Fossom, L. H., & Sparber, S. B. (1977). !b-Endorphin: Dose-dependent suppression of fixed-ratio operant behavior: Life Sciences Vol 21(7) Oct 1977, 927-932.
  • Lillo Jover, J. (1984). Latent inhibition and contextual change: Novelty or variation? : Informes de Psicologia Vol 3(1-2) 1984, 71-81.
  • Lin, C.-s., & Chang, W.-j. (1975). Suppression of high-amplitude GSR activity through avoidance conditioning: Bulletin of Educational Psychology Vol 8 Jun 1975, 45-59.
  • Lin, W., Wei, X., Guo, Y., Tang, C., & Liu, Y. (1998). Study on conditioned immunosuppression and conditioned taste aversion: Acta Psychologica Sinica Vol 30(4) 1998, 418-422.
  • Lindewall, D. G. (1982). The effect of several schedule of reinforcement variables on the behavioral changes produced by the Estes-Skinner procedure: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Linton, L., & Roberts, A. E. (1974). Changes in unsignalled avoidance behavior when a pre-shock CS is response-produced: Psychological Reports Vol 35(3) Dec 1974, 1251-1258.
  • Lipp, O. V., & Vaitl, D. (1992). Latent inhibition in human Pavlovian differential conditioning: Effect of additional stimulation after preexposure and relation to schizotypal traits: Personality and Individual Differences Vol 13(9) Sep 1992, 1003-1012.
  • Livesey, D. (1988). A maturation effect in response inhibition in the rat: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 50(2) Sep 1988, 153-163.
  • Locurto, C. M., Norris, J., Cataldo, P. F., & Laplace, A. (1981). Time allocation and positive conditioned suppression: Behaviour Analysis Letters Vol 1(2) Mar 1981, 123-130.
  • Locurto, C. M., Tierney, J., & Fitzgerald, S. (1981). Omission training and positive conditioned suppression in the rat: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 9(2) May 1981, 261-268.
  • Loew, E. R., & Sillman, A. J. (1998). An action spectrum for the light-dependent inhibition of swimming behavior in newly hatched white sturgeon, Acipenser transmontanus: Vision Research Vol 38(1) Jan 1998, 111-114.
  • Logan, G. D., & Irwin, D. E. (2000). Don't look! Don't touch! Inhibitory control of eye and hand movements: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 7(1) Mar 2000, 107-112.
  • Logan, M. S., & Schnur, P. (1976). The effects of nonreinforced and randomly reinforced stimulus preexposure on conditioned suppression in rats: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 8(4) Oct 1976, 336-338.
  • Losa, E. G. (1999). Self-conditioned suppression in parenteral opiate drug addicts: Revista Psiquis Vol 20(4) 1999, 47-50.
  • Loskutova, L. V., Lukyanenko, F. Y., & Ilyutchenok, R. Y. (1989). Interaction of serotonin- and dopaminergic brain systems in mechanisms of latent inhibition in rats: Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel'nosti Vol 39(4) 1989, 714-720.
  • Lovibond, P. F., Preston, G. C., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1984). Context specificity of conditioning, extinction, and latent inhibition: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 10(3) Jul 1984, 360-375.
  • Lowe, M. R., Davis, W., Lucks, D., Annunziato, R., & Butryn, M. (2006). Weight suppression predicts weight gain during inpatient treatment of bulimia nervosa: Physiology & Behavior Vol 87(3) Mar 2006, 487-492.
  • Lubow, R. E. (1989). Latent inhibition and conditioned attention theory. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Lubow, R. E., Alek, M., & Arzy, J. (1975). Behavioral decrement following stimulus preexposure: Effects of number of preexposures, presence of a second stimulus, and interstimulus interval in children and adults: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 1(2) Apr 1975, 178-188.
  • Lubow, R. E., Wagner, M., & Weiner, I. (1982). The effects of compound stimulus preexposure of two elements differing in salience on the acquisition of conditioned suppression: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 10(4) Nov 1982, 483-489.
  • Lukas, S. E., Mello, N. K., Bree, M. P., & Mendelson, J. H. (1988). Differential tolerance development to buprenorphine-, diprenorphine-, and heroin-induced disruption of food-maintained responding in Macaque monkeys: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 30(4) Aug 1988, 977-982.
  • Lyon, D. O. (1964). Some notes on conditioned suppression and reinforcement schedules: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 7(4) 1964, 289-291.
  • Lyon, D. O., & Felton, M. (1966). Conditioned suppression and variable ratio reinforcement: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 9(3) 1966, 245-248.
  • Lyons, D. M., Lopez, J. M., Yang, C., & Schatzberg, A. F. (2000). Stress-level cortisol treatment impairs inhibitory control of behavior in monkeys: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 20(20) Oct 2000, 7816-7821.
  • Lysle, D. T., Cunnick, J. E., Fowler, H., & Rabin, B. S. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning of shock-induced suppression of lymphocyte reactivity: Acquisition, extinction, and preexposure effects: Life Sciences Vol 42(22) 1988, 2185-2194.
  • Lysle, D. T., & Fowler, H. (1985). Inhibition as a "slave" process: Deactivation of conditioned inhibition through extinction of conditioned excitation: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 11(1) Jan 1985, 71-94.
  • MacDonald, L. (1973). The relative aversiveness of signalled versus unsignalled shock-punishment: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 20(1) Jul 1973, 37-46.
  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). Blocking of conditioned suppression: Role of the first compound trial: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 1(4) Oct 1975, 335-345.
  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1976). Overshadowing and stimulus intensity: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 4(2) May 1976, 186-192.
  • Mackintosh, N. J., Bygrave, D. J., & Picton, B. M. (1977). Locus of the effect of a surprising reinforcer in the attenuation of blocking: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 29(2) May 1977, 327-336.
  • Mackintosh, N. J., Dickinson, A., & Cotton, M. M. (1980). Surprise and blocking: Effects of the number of compound trials: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 8(3) Aug 1980, 387-391.
  • Mackintosh, N. J., & Reese, B. (1979). One-trial overshadowing: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 31(3) Aug 1979, 519-526.
  • Maes, J. H. R., & Vossen, J. M. H. (1996). Differential inhibition using contextual stimuli: Behavioural Processes Vol 37(2-3) Sep 1996, 167-184.
  • Mahoney, W. J. (1979). Inhibition and blocking with temporal cues in conditioned suppression of barpressing: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Mahoney, W. J., & Ayres, J. J. (1976). One-trial simultaneous and backward fear conditioning as reflected in conditioned suppression of licking in rats: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 4(4) Nov 1976, 357-362.
  • Maisiak, R., & Frey, P. W. (1977). Second-order conditioning: The importance of stimulus overlap on second-order trials: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 5(3) Aug 1977, 309-314.
  • Maletzky, B. M., & Steinhauser, C. (2003). "Olfactory aversion: Notes on procedure, with speculations on its mechanism of effect": Comment: Sexual Abuse: Journal of Research and Treatment Vol 15(3) Jul 2003, 215-217.
  • Maltzman, I. (1990). Locus of semantic generalization of the galvanic skin response and possible inhibitory influence of conditional stimuli upon unconditional response following conditioning with innocuous and noxious unconditional stimuli: Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science Vol 25(2) Apr-Jun 1990, 63-76.
  • Manganotti, P., Patuzzo, S., Cortese, F., Palermo, A., Smania, N., & Fiaschi, A. (2002). Motor disinhibition in affected and unaffected hemisphere in the early period of recovery after stroke: Clinical Neurophysiology Vol 113(6) Jun 2002, 936-943.
  • Marczinski, C. A., & Fillmore, M. T. (2003). Preresponse cues reduce the impairing effects of alcohol on the execution and suppression of responses: Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology Vol 11(1) Feb 2003, 110-117.
  • Marshall, L. B., & Smith, O. A. (1975). Prefrontal control of conditioned suppression and associated cardiovascular variables in the monkey (Macaca mulatta): Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 88(1) Jan 1975, 21-35.
  • Marshall, L. B., Zeiner, A. R., & Smith, O. A. (1978). Development of conditioned suppression and associated cardiovascular responses in the monkey: Physiology & Behavior Vol 20(4) Apr 1978, 441-446.
  • Martin, R. B. (1984). Punishment-induced facilitation and suppression in the shuttlebox: Psychological Record Vol 34(3) Sum 1984, 381-388.
  • Martinez, G. A., Aguado Aguilar, L., & Rosales, G.-H. (1989). Sensory preconditioning: Implications for a theory of conditioned inhibition: Revista de Psicologia General y Aplicada Vol 42(3) Jul 1989, 293-298.
  • Maser, J. D., Gallup, G. G., & Barnhill, R. (1973). Conditioned inhibition and tonic immobility: Stimulus control of an innate fear response in the chicken: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 83(1) Apr 1973, 128-133.
  • Mast, M., Blanchard, R. J., & Blanchard, D. C. (1982). The relationship of freezing and response suppression in a CER situation: Psychological Record Vol 32(2) Spr 1982, 151-167.
  • Mast, M. S. (1978). A behavioral analysis of Pavlovian fear conditioning in relation to response suppression in the conditioned emotional response: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Mathews, A., & Wertheimer, M. (1958). A "pure" measure of perceptual defense uncontaminated by response suppression: The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology Vol 57(3) Nov 1958, 373-376.
  • Matsumiya, Y., & Kling, J. W. (1970). Conditioned suppression based on positively reinforcing intracranial stimulation: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 12(1) May 1970, 26-35.
  • Matthews, T. J., McHugh, T. G., & Carr, L. D. (1974). Pavlovian and instrumental determinants of response suppression in the pigeon: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 87(3) Sep 1974, 500-506.
  • Matzel, L. D., Brown, A. M., & Miller, R. R. (1987). Associative effects of US preexposure: Modulation of conditioned responding by an excitatory training context: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 13(1) Jan 1987, 65-72.
  • Matzel, L. D., Gladstein, L., & Miller, R. R. (1988). Conditioned excitation and conditioned inhibition are not mutually exclusive: Learning and Motivation Vol 19(2) May 1988, 99-121.
  • Matzel, L. D., Held, F. P., & Miller, R. R. (1988). Information and expression of simultaneous and backward associations: Implications for contiguity theory: Learning and Motivation Vol 19(4) Nov 1988, 317-344.
  • Matzel, L. D., Schachtman, T. R., & Miller, R. R. (1988). Learned irrelevance exceeds the sum of CS-preexposure and US-preexposure deficits: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 14(3) Jul 1988, 311-319.
  • May, C. P., Kane, M. J., & Hasher, L. (1995). Determinants of negative priming: Psychological Bulletin Vol 118(1) Jul 1995, 35-54.
  • McCloskey, T. C., Paul, B. K., & Commissaris, R. L. (1987). Buspirone effects in an animal conflict procedure: Comparison with diazepam and phenobarbital: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 27(1) May 1987, 171-175.
  • McCoy, D. F., & et al. (1986). Some parameters of conditioned immunosuppression: Species difference and CS-US delay: Physiology & Behavior Vol 36(4) 1986, 731-736.
  • McDougall, S. A. (1988). Effects of sulpiride and LY 171555 on the ontogeny of response suppression learning in preweanling rat pups: The role of dopamine D-2 receptors and intraneuronal dopamine metabolites: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • McDougall, S. A., & Nonneman, A. J. (1989). Dopamine D-2 receptor mediation of response suppression learning of young rats: Developmental Psychobiology Vol 22(3) Apr 1989, 235-256.
  • McDougall, S. A., Zolman, J. F., & Mattingly, B. A. (1987). Effects of apomorphine and haloperidol on response suppression learning of young chicks: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 101(2) Apr 1987, 254-263.
  • McGaugh, J. L., & Petrinovich, L. F. (1966). Neural consolidation and electroconvulsive shock reexamined: Psychological Review Vol 73(4) Jul 1966, 382-387.
  • McGlynn, F. D., Miller, W. B., & Fancher, J. (1975). Response elimination among chronic psychiatric patients: Extinction versus omission training: Psychological Record Vol 25(2) Spr 1975, 181-186.
  • McIntire, R. W. (1966). Conditioned suppression and self-stimulation: Psychonomic Science Vol 5(7) 1966, 273-274.
  • McIntire, R. W. (1966). Extinction of the freezing component of the CER after frontal ablation: Psychological Reports 19(1) 1966, 187-193.
  • McIntosh, A. R., & Gonzalez-Lima, F. (1994). Network interactions among limbic cortices, basal forebrain, and cerebellum differentiate a tone conditioned as a Pavlovian excitor or inhibitor: Fluorodeoxyglucose mapping and covariance structural modeling: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 72(4) Oct 1994, 1717-1733.
  • McKinzie, D. L., Lee, J., Bronfen, J. H., Spear, L. P., & et al. (1994). Context and tone conditioning are selectively impaired by ethanol in the preweanling rat: Effects of dose and time of administration: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 62(3) Nov 1994, 201-209.
  • McMichael, J. S. (1966). Incubation of anxiety and instrumental behavior: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 61(2) Apr 1966, 208-211.
  • McNaughton, N. (1990). Action of septal lesions on facilitation rather than inhibition of responding in conditioned suppression: New Zealand Journal of Psychology Vol 19(1) Jun 1990, 31-34.
  • McNeese, R. R. (1977). Conditioned suppression in the rat as a behavioral assay for conspecific odor: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • McPhee, J. E., Rauhut, A. S., & Ayres, J. J. B. (2001). Evidence for learning-deficit versus performance-deficit theories of latent inhibition in Pavlovian fear conditioning: Learning and Motivation Vol 32(3) Aug 2001, 274-305.
  • McSweeney, F. K. (1987). Suppression by reinforcement, a model for multiple-schedule behavioral contrast: Behavioural Processes Vol 15(2-3) Dec 1987, 191-209.
  • Meachum, C. L. (1990). The role of response-contingent incentives in lithium chloride-mediated suppression of an operant response: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 42(2-B) May 1990, 175-195.
  • Melchers, K. G., Wolff, S., & Lachnit, H. (2006). Extinction of conditioned inhibition through nonreinforced presentation of the inhibitor: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 13(4) Aug 2006, 662-667.
  • Mello e Souza, T., Roesler, R., Madruga, M., de-Paris, F., Quevedo, J., Rodrigues, C., et al. (1999). Differential effects of post-training muscimol and AP5 infusions into different regions of the cingulate cortex on retention for inhibitory avoidance in rats: Neurobiology of Learning and Memory Vol 72(2) Sep 1999, 118-127.
  • Melo, L. L., Brandao, M. L., Graeff, F. G., & Sandner, G. (1997). Bilateral ablation of the auditory cortex in the rat alters conditioned emotional suppression to a sound as appraised through a latent inhibition study: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 88(1) Oct 1997, 59-65.
  • Meltzer, D. (1988). Positive conditioned suppression after shifts in CS duration and US probability: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 26(6) Nov 1988, 565-568.
  • Meltzer, D., & Niebuhr, B. R. (1974). Additive and suppressive response summation with a chain schedule: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 22(3) Nov 1974, 519-524.
  • Meltzer, H. (1936). Review of Stammering and Allied Disorders: Journal of Educational Psychology Vol 27(6) Sep 1936, 479-480.
  • Memmott, J., & Reberg, D. (1977). Differential conditioned suppression in the Konorski-Lawicka paradigm: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 5(2) May 1977, 124-128.
  • Merlot, E., Moze, E., Dantzer, R., & Neveu, P. J. (2002). Suppression of restraint-induced plasma cytokines in mice pretreated with LPS: Stress: The International Journal on the Biology of Stress Vol 5(2) 2002, 131-135.
  • Merrill, H. K. (1976). Conditioned suppression in rats responding for hypothalamic brain stimulation: Physiological Psychology Vol 4(2) Jun 1976, 151-154.
  • Meyer, J. S. (1975). An investigation of adrenocorticotropic hormone influences on the acquisition and retention of conditioned avoidance behavior by rats: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Miczek, K. A. (1973). Effects of scopolamine, amphetamine and benzodiazepines on conditioned suppression: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 1(4) Jul 1973, 401-411.
  • Miczek, K. A. (1973). Effects of scopolamine, amphetamine and chlordiazepoxide on punishment: Psychopharmacologia Vol 28(4) 1973, 373-389.
  • Miczek, K. A., & Luttinger, D. (1978). Differential attenuation of two kinds of conditioned suppression by d-amphetamine and pentobarbital: Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics Vol 205(2) May 1978, 282-290.
  • Migo, E. M., Corbett, K., Graham, J., Smith, S., Tate, S., Moran, P. M., et al. (2006). A novel test of conditioned inhibition correlates with personality measures of schizotypy and reward sensitivity: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 168(2) Mar 2006, 299-306.
  • Milby, J. B. (1973). Pre- and post-extinction responding as a function of extinction duration: Psychological Reports Vol 32(3, Pt 1) Jun 1973, 843-847.
  • Millenson, J. R., & MacMillan, A. S. (1975). Abortive responding during punishment of bar holding: Learning and Motivation Vol 6(2) May 1975, 279-288.
  • Miller, J. S., McKinzie, D. L., Kraebel, K. S., & Spear, N. E. (1996). Changes in the expression of stimulus selection: Blocking represents selective memory retrieval rather than selective associations: Learning and Motivation Vol 27(3) Aug 1996, 307-316.
  • Miller, R. R., Esposito, J. J., & Grahame, N. J. (1992). Overshadowing-like effects between potential comparator stimuli: Covariation in comparator roles of context and punctate excitor used in inhibitory training as a function of excitor salience: Learning and Motivation Vol 23(1) Feb 1992, 1-26.
  • Miller, R. R., Hallam, S. C., Hong, J. Y., & Dufore, D. S. (1991). Associative structure of differential inhibition: Implications for models of conditioned inhibition: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 17(2) Apr 1991, 141-150.
  • Ming, Z., Caiqi, C., & Yang, Z. (2005). The Influence of Inhibition of Return on the Storage and Goal Maintenance in Working Memory: Psychological Science (China) Vol 28(2) Mar 2005, 281-284.
  • Mitchell, C., & Heyes, C. (1996). Simultaneous overshadowing and potentiation of taste and contextual cues by a second taste in toxicosis conditioning: Learning and Motivation Vol 27(1) Feb 1996, 58-72.
  • Mitchell, S. H. (2004). Measuring Impulsivity and Modeling Its Association With Cigarette Smoking: Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews Vol 3(4) Dec 2004, 261-275.
  • Mitchell, S. H. (2005). "Measuring Impulsivity and Modeling Its Association With Cigarette Smoking": Erratum: Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews Vol 4(2) Jun 2005, 137.
  • Miyake, A., Emerson, M. J., Padilla, F., & Ahn, J.-c. (2004). Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: The effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm: Acta Psychologica Vol 115(2-3) Feb-Mar 2004, 123-142.
  • Mokler, D. J., & Rech, R. H. (1985). Mechanisms of the initial treatment phenomenon to diazepam in the rat: Psychopharmacology Vol 87(2) Oct 1985, 242-246.
  • Monastra, V. J. (1978). Reacquisition of conditioned suppression to visual and auditory stimuli after extinction to the stimuli individually or in compound: An investigation of the theoretical and therapeutic implications of the Rescorla-Wagner Learning Theory: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Monleon, S., Urquiza, A., Arenas, M. C., Vinader-Caerols, C., & Parra, A. (2002). Chronic administration of fluoxetine impairs inhibitory avoidance in male but not female mice: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 136(2) Nov 2002, 483-488.
  • Montgomery, K. C. (1951). An experimental investigation of reactive inhibition and conditioned inhibition: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 41(1) Jan 1951, 39-51.
  • Monti, P. M. (1975). Residual fear of the CS as a function of response prevention after avoidance or classical-defensive conditioning in the rat: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Moore, J. W., Yeo, C. H., Oakley, D. A., & Russell, S. (1980). Conditioned inhibition of the nictitating membrane response in decorticate rabbits: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 1(5) Oct 1980, 397-409.
  • Moores, E., & Andrade, J. (2000). Ability of dyslexic and control teenagers to sustain attention and inhibit responses: European Journal of Cognitive Psychology Vol 12(4) Dec 2000, 520-540.
  • Moran, P. M., Fischer, T. R., Hitchcock, J. M., & Moser, P. C. (1996). Effects of clozapine on latent inhibition in the rat: Behavioural Pharmacology Vol 7(1) Jan 1996, 42-48.
  • Moran, P. M., & Moser, P. C. (1992). MDL 73,147EF, a 5-HT-sub-3 antagonist, facilitates latent inhibition in the rat: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 42(3) Jul 1992, 519-522.
  • Morii, Y., & Karakawa, C. (1996). Counting of sequentially presented electrical shocks in rats: Japanese Journal of Animal Psychology Vol 46(1) Jul 1996, 21-28.
  • Morris, D. D. (1966). Threshold for conditioned suppression using X-rays as the preaversive stimulus: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 9(1) 1966, 29-34.
  • Morris, M. D., & Gebhart, G. F. (1978). The effect of morphine on fear extinction in rats: Psychopharmacology Vol 57(3) 1978, 267-271.
  • Morris, R. C., Wookey, P. E., & Kirby, R. M. (1979). Effects of signalling reinforcement which maintains variable ratio performance of rats and pigeons: Psychological Reports Vol 44(3, Pt 1) Jun 1979, 843-852.
  • Morrison, G. R., & Collyer, R. (1974). Taste-mediated conditioned aversion to an exteroceptive stimulus following LiCl poisoning: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 86(1) Jan 1974, 51-55.
  • Moser, P. C., Moran, P. M., Frank, R. A., & Kehne, J. H. (1995). Reversal of amphetamine-induced behaviours by MDL 100,907, a selective 5-HT-sub(2A ) antagonist: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 73(1-2) Dec 1995, 163-167.
  • Mowrer, R. R. (1987). Latent inhibition of contextual stimuli reduces the US preexposure effect in rats: Psychological Record Vol 37(2) Spr 1987, 239-245.
  • Mulholland, T. B. (1979). A proposed feedback method for studying the inhibition of EEG responses to visual stimuli: Psychophysiology Vol 16(1) Jan 1979, 61-65.
  • Mulick, J. A. (1975). Use of positive reinforcement contingencies to suppress behavior during extinction: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Mulick, J. A., Leitenberg, H., & Rawson, R. A. (1976). Alternative response training, differential renforcement of other behavior, and extinction in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus): Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 25(3) May 1976, 311-320.
  • Mulvaney, D. E., Dinsmoor, J. A., Jwaideh, A. R., & Hughes, L. H. (1974). Punishment of observing by the negative discriminative stimulus: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 21(1) Jan 1974, 37-44.
  • Muscat, R., & Willner, P. (1992). Suppression of sucrose drinking by chronic mild unpredictable stress: A methodological analysis: Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews Vol 16(4) Win 1992, 507-517.
  • Myer, J. S. (1973). Effects of punishing elements of a simple instrumental-consummatory response chain: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 19(2) Mar 1973, 251-257.
  • Myer, J. S. (1975). Shock-induced suppression and potentiation of instrumental responding by rats: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 88(2) Feb 1975, 580-585.
  • Myers, K. M., & Davis, M. (2004). AX+, BX- Discrimination Learning in the Fear-Potentiated Startle Paradigm: Possible Relevance to Inhibitory Fear Learning in Extinction: Learning & Memory Vol 11(4) Jul-Aug 2004, 464-475.
  • Myslivecek, J. (1982). Differential development of various forms of higher nervous activity in the ontogeny: Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel'nosti Vol 32(1) 1982, 39-46.
  • Myslivecek, J. (1982). Genetic and nutritional factors in the development of inhibitory learning: Activitas Nervosa Superior Vol 24(3) 1982, 128-129.
  • Nabeshima, T., Kamei, H., & Kameyama, T. (1988). A role played by sigma receptors in the conditioned suppression of motility in mice: Psychopharmacology Vol 94(4) Apr 1988, 515-520.
  • Nabeshima, T., Katoh, A., Hiramatsu, M., & Kameyama, T. (1986). A role played by dopamine and opioid neuronal systems in stress-induced motor suppression (conditioned suppression of motility) in mice: Brain Research Vol 398(2) Nov 1986, 354-360.
  • Nabeshima, T., Katoh, A., & Kameyama, T. (1988). Inhibition of enkephalin degradation attenuated stress-induced motor suppression (conditioned suppression of motility): Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics Vol 244(1) Jan 1988, 303-309.
  • Nabeshima, T., Noda, Y., & Kameyama, T. (1988). GABAergic modulation of memory with regard to passive avoidance and conditioned suppression tasks in mice: Psychopharmacology Vol 94(1) Jan 1988, 69-73.
  • Nageishi, Y., & Imada, H. (1974). Suppression of licking behavior in rats as a function of predictability of shock and probability of conditioned-stimulus-shock pairings: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 87(6) Dec 1974, 1165-1173.
  • Nakajima, M., Nakajima, S., & Imada, H. (1999). General learned irrelevance and its prevention: Learning and Motivation Vol 30(4) Nov 1999, 265-280.
  • Nakajima, S. (1997). Failure of inhibition by B over C after A+, AB-, ABC+ training: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 23(4) Oct 1997, 482-490.
  • Nakajima, S., Takahashi, K., & Blaisdell, A. P. (2006). An assessment of context-specificity of the CS-preexposure effect in Pavlovian excitatory and inhibitory conditioning: Behavioural Processes Vol 73(1) Aug 2006, 84-91.
  • Nakajima, S., & Urushihara, K. (1999). Inhibition and facilitation by B over C after A+, AB-, and ABC+ training with multimodality stimulus combinations: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 25(1) Jan 1999, 68-81.
  • Napolitano, F., de Rosa, G., Grasso, F., Migliori, G., & Bordi, A. (1998). Conditioned inhibition of antibody response and CD4 positive cells: Physiology & Behavior Vol 64(3) Jun 1998, 395-398.
  • Nation, J. R., Bourgeois, A. E., & Clark, D. E. (1983). Behavioral effects of chronic lead exposure in the adult rat: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 18(6) Jun 1983, 833-840.
  • Nation, J. R., Bourgeois, A. E., Clark, D. E., & Hare, M. F. (1983). The effects of chronic cobalt exposure on behavior and metallothionein levels in the adult rat: Neurobehavioral Toxicology & Teratology Vol 5(1) Jan-Feb 1983, 9-15.
  • Nation, J. R., Clark, D. E., & Baker, D. M. (1983). The effects of chronic cadmium exposure on schedule controlled responding and conditioned suppression in the adult rat: Neurobehavioral Toxicology & Teratology Vol 5(3) May-Jun 1983, 275-282.
  • Neito, J., & Boakes, R. A. (1983). The effects of horn devaluation on the transfer of conditioned inhibition between horn and shock: Revista Mexicana de Analisis de la Conducta Vol 9(1-2) Jun-Dec 1983, 145-162.
  • Nelson, J. B., & Bouton, M. E. (1997). The effects of a context switch following serial and simultaneous feature-negative discriminations: Learning and Motivation Vol 28(1) Feb 1997, 56-84.
  • Nelson, J. B., & Sanjuan, M. d. C. (2006). A context-specific latent inhibition effect in a human conditioned suppression task: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 59(6) Jun 2006, 1003-1020.
  • Nelson, K. J. (1987). Conditioned inhibition from incomplete reductions in the probability of reinforcement: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 39(4-B) Nov 1987, 365-391.
  • Nelson, S., Toth, L., Sheth, B., & Sur, M. (1994). Orientation selectivity of cortical neurons during intracellular blockade of inhibition: Science Vol 265(5173) Aug 1994, 774-777.
  • Neuenschwander, N., Fabrigoule, C., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1987). Fear of the warning signal during overtraining of avoidance: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 39B(1) Feb 1987, 23-33.
  • Neuenschwander-El Massioui, N., Dutrieux, G., & Edeline, J.-M. (1991). Conditioned hippocampal cellular response to a behaviorally silent conditioned stimulus: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 105(2) Apr 1991, 313-325.
  • Neumann, D. L. (2006). The effects of physical context changes and multiple extinction contexts on two forms of renewal in a conditioned suppression task with humans: Learning and Motivation Vol 37(2) May 2006, 149-175.
  • Neumann, D. L. (2007). The resistance of renewal to instructions that devalue the role of contextual cues in a conditioned suppression task with humans: Learning and Motivation Vol 38(2) May 2007, 105-127.
  • Neumann, D. L., Lipp, O. V., & Siddle, D. A. T. (1997). Conditioned inhibition of autonomic Pavlovian conditioning in humans: Biological Psychology Vol 46(3) Oct 1997, 223-233.
  • Neveu, P. J., & Le Moal, M. (1987). Conditioned immunosuppression: A new methodological approach. New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences.
  • Newman, L. S., Duff, K. J., Hedberg, D. A., & Blitstein, J. (1996). Rebound effects in impression formation: Assimilation and contrast effects following thought suppression: Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Vol 32(5) Sep 1996, 460-483.
  • Nicholson, D. A., & Freeman, J. H., Jr. (2002). Neuronal correlates of conditioned inhibition of the eyeblink response in the anterior inerpositus nucleus: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 116(1) Feb 2002, 22-36.
  • Niebuhr, B. R. (1976). The effects of septal lesions on positive conditioned suppression and positive conditioned enhancement: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Nielsen, E. B., & Appel, J. B. (1985). The effect of drugs on the acquisition of stimulus control in a conditioned suppression procedure: Psychopharmacology Vol 85(1) Jan 1985, 80-86.
  • Nienhuys, T. G., & Caelli, T. M. (1978). Determination of operating characteristics of cats using conditioned suppression: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 32(3) Sep 1978, 193-197.
  • Nieto, J. (1984). Transfer of conditioned inhibition across different aversive reinforcers in the rat: Learning and Motivation Vol 15(1) Feb 1984, 37-57.
  • Nolan, B. C. (2005). Neurobiological mechanisms of conditioned excitation and conditioned inhibition of the eyeblink response in rats. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Nolan, B. C., & Freeman, J. H., Jr. (2005). Purkinje Cell Loss by OX7-Saporin Impairs Excitatory and Inhibitory Eyeblink Conditioning: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 119(1) Feb 2005, 190-201.
  • Nolan, B. C., Nicholson, D. A., & Freeman, J. H. (2002). Blockade of GABA-sub(A ) receptors in the interpositus nucleus modulates expression of conditioned excitation but not conditioned inhibition of the eyeblink response: Integrative Physiological & Behavioral Science Vol 37(4) Oct-Dec 2002, 293-310.
  • Oberling, P., Gunther, L. M., & Miller, R. R. (1999). Latent inhibition and learned irrelevance of occasion setting: Learning and Motivation Vol 30(2) May 1999, 157-182.
  • O'Boyle, E. A., & Bouton, M. E. (1996). Conditioned inhibition in a multiple-category learning task: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 49B(1) Feb 1996, 1-23.
  • O'Boyle, M. (1974). Conditioned suppression of mouse killing in rats following radiation or lithium chloride injections: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • O'Boyle, M. (1976). Suppression of mouse-killing in rats following irradiation: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 42(2) Apr 1976, 511-514.
  • Ohki, Y., & Imada, H. (1984). The effects of intensities of shock and thirst on licking conditioned suppression: Japanese Psychological Research Vol 26(1) 1984, 32-41.
  • Oley, N. H. (1974). Free-operant avoidance, punishment and conditioned suppression in cats with septal lesions: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (1998). Effects of reward and response cost on response inhibition in AD/HD, disruptive, anxious, and normal children: Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology Vol 26(3) Jun 1998, 161-174.
  • Orme-Johnson, D. W., & Yarczower, M. (1974). Conditioned suppression, punishment, and aversion: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 21(1) Jan 1974, 57-74.
  • Overton, D. A., Shen, C. F., & Tatham, T. A. (1993). Centrally acting drugs act as conditioned stimuli in a conditioned suppression of drinking task: Psychopharmacology Vol 112(2-3) Sep 1993, 270-276.
  • Padmanabhapillai, A., Porjesz, B., Ranganathan, M., Jones, K. A., Chorlian, D. B., Tang, Y., et al. (2006). Suppression of early evoked gamma band response in male alcoholics during a visual oddball task: International Journal of Psychophysiology Vol 60(1) Apr 2006, 15-26.
  • Pallares, M. A., Nadal, R. A., & Ferre, N. S. (1992). Effects of oral ethanol self-administration on the inhibition of the lever-press response in rats: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 43(2) Oct 1992, 589-595.
  • Palmer, A. R., & Harrison, R. V. (1985). Suppression by tones of the click evoked compound action potential in the normal and pathological guinea-pig cochlea and in man: Scandinavian Audiology Vol 14(2) 1985, 67-74.
  • Panlilio, L. V., Weiss, S. J., & Schindler, C. W. (2000). Stimulus compounding enhances conditioned suppression produced by cocaine-paired stimuli: Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology Vol 8(1) Feb 2000, 6-13.
  • Papini, M. R., & Bitterman, M. E. (1993). The two-test strategy in the study of inhibitory conditioning: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 19(4) Oct 1993, 342-352.
  • Papini, M. R., & Pellegrini, S. (2006). Scaling relative incentive value in consummatory behavior: Learning and Motivation Vol 37(4) Nov 2006, 357-378.
  • Parisi, T., & Ison, J. R. (1981). Ontogeny of control over the acoustic startle reflex by visual prestimulation in the rat: Developmental Psychobiology Vol 14(4) Jul 1981, 311-316.
  • Park, L. I. (1972). Adrenalectomy, gonadectomy, and TACE: Effects on conditioned suppression in rats: Dissertation Abstracts International Vol.
  • Parker, L. A. (1979). Suppression of consummatory behavior elicited by a lithium-conditioned flavor: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Parker, L. A. (1980). Conditioned suppression of drinking: A measure of the CR elicited by a lithium-conditioned flavor: Learning and Motivation Vol 11(4) Nov 1980, 538-559.
  • Pearce, J. M., & Dickinson, A. (1975). Pavlovian countercondition: Changing the suppressive properties of shock by association with food: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 1(2) Apr 1975, 170-177.
  • Pearce, J. M., & Kaye, H. (1985). Strength of the orienting response during inhibitory conditioning: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 11(3) Jul 1985, 405-420.
  • Pearce, J. M., Nicholas, D. J., & Dickinson, A. (1981). The potentiation effect during serial conditioning: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 33B(3) Aug 1981, 159-179.
  • Pearce, J. M., Nicholas, D. J., & Dickinson, A. (1982). Loss of associability by a conditioned inhibitor: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 34B(3) Aug 1982, 149-162.
  • Pearce, J. M., & Redhead, E. S. (1995). Supernormal conditioning: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 21(2) Apr 1995, 155-165.
  • Pearce, J. M., & Wilson, P. N. (1991). Failure of excitatory conditioning to extinguish the influence of a conditioned inhibitor: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 17(4) Oct 1991, 519-529.
  • Pecka, M., Zahn, T. P., Saunier-Rebori, B., Siveke, I., Felmy, F., Wiegrebe, L., et al. (2007). Inhibiting the inhibition: A neuronal network for sound localization in reverberant environments: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 27(7) Feb 2007, 1782-1790.
  • Pellon, R., Garcia, J. M., & Sanchez, P. (1995). Blocking and electrodermal conditioning in humans: Psicologica Vol 16(3) 1995, 321-329.
  • Pennypacker, H. S. (1964). External inhibition of the conditioned eyelid reflex: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 67(1) Jan 1964, 33-40.
  • Persinger, M. A. (1985). Death anxiety as a semantic conditioned suppression paradigm: Perceptual and Motor Skills Vol 60(3) Jun 1985, 827-830.
  • Persinger, M. A., Lafreniere, G. F., & Falter, H. (1976). Oral taurine effects on inhibitory behavior: Response transients to step-like schedule changes: Psychopharmacology Vol 49(3) 1976, 249-252.
  • Persinger, M. A., & Pear, J. J. (1972). Prenatal exposure to an E L F-rotating magnetic field and subsequent increase in conditioned suppression: Developmental Psychobiology Vol 5(3) 1972, 269-274.
  • Pert, A., & Avis, H. H. (1974). Dissociation between scopolamine and mecamylamine during fear conditioning in rats: Physiological Psychology Vol 2(2) Jun 1974, 111-116.
  • Pertovaara, A., & Kalmari, J. (2002). Neuropathy reduces viscero-somatic inhibition via segmental mechanisms in rats: Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research Vol 13(8) Jun 2002, 1047-1050.
  • Pierson, S. C. (1972). Conditioned suppression to odorous stimuli in the rat: Dissertation Abstracts International Vol.
  • Pierson, S. C. (1974). Conditioned suppression to odorous stimuli in the rat: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 86(4) Apr 1974, 708-717.
  • Pineno, O. (2007). A response rule for positive and negative stimulus interaction in associative learning and performance: Psychonomic Bulletin & Review Vol 14(6) Dec 2007, 1115-1124.
  • Pineno, O., Urushihara, K., & Miller, R. R. (2005). Spontaneous Recovery From Forward and Backward Blocking: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 31(2) Apr 2005, 172-183.
  • Pineno, O., Urushihara, K., Stout, S., Fuss, J., & Miller, R. R. (2006). When more is less: Extending training of the blocking association following compound training attenuates the blocking effect: Learning & Behavior Vol 34(1) Feb 2006, 21-36.
  • Platt, D. M., Rodefer, J. S., Rowlett, J. K., & Spealman, R. D. (2003). Suppression of cocaine- and food-maintained behavior by the D2-like receptor partial agonist terguride in squirrel monkeys: Psychopharmacology Vol 166(3) Mar 2003, 298-305.
  • Poling, A., & Thompson, T. (1977). Suppression of ethanol-reinforced level pressing by delaying food availability: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 28(3) Nov 1977, 271-283.
  • Poling, A., Urbain, C., & Thompson, T. (1977). Effects of d-amphetamine and chlordiazepoxide on positive conditioned suppression: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 7(3) Sep 1977, 233-237.
  • Poltyreva, T. E., & Petrov, E. S. (1983). Influence of previous experience in rats' reactions to danger signal on their behaviour in the "open field." Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel'nosti Vol 33(4) Jun-Aug 1983, 679-684.
  • Poplawsky, A., & Calabro-Robbins, S. (1989). The effects of lateral, medial, or complete septal lesions on positive conditioned suppression in rats: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 51(3) May 1989, 377-388.
  • Popova, E. I., & Koptelova, I. A. (1982). Prognostication by man of readiness to voluntarily inhibit fear in anticipation of a probable presentation of stress stimuli: Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel'nosti Vol 32(6) 1982, 1053-1060.
  • Poulos, C. X., & Hinson, R. E. (1984). A homeostatic model of Pavlovian conditioning: Tolerance to scopolamine-induced adipsia: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 10(1) Jan 1984, 75-89.
  • Powell, J., Dawkins, L., & Davis, R. E. (2002). Smoking, reward responsiveness, and response inhibition: Tests of an incentive motivation model: Biological Psychiatry Vol 51(2) Jan 2002, 151-163.
  • Powell, M. L. (1976). Punished avoidance: An experimental analogue to clinical self-injury: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Provost, S. C. (1990). Excitatory and inhibitory backward conditioning in the rat: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Purves, D., Bonardi, C., & Hall, G. (1995). Enhancement of latent inhibition in rats with electrolytic lesions of the hippocampus: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 109(2) Apr 1995, 366-370.
  • Quartermain, D., Judge, M. E., & Jung, H. (1988). Amphetamine enhances retrieval following diverse sources of forgetting: Physiology & Behavior Vol 43(2) 1988, 239-241.
  • Quirk, G. J. (2007). Prefrontal-Amygdala Interactions in the Regulation of Fear. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Quirt, B. M., & Cohen, J. S. (1974). Effects of intermittent and constant punishment on resistance to punishment in a free operant situation for the albino rat: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 87(3) Sep 1974, 539-547.
  • Quy, R. J., & Kubiak, E. W. (1974). A comparison between "aware" and "naive" conditions in the suppression of GSR activity: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 26(4) Nov 1974, 561-565.
  • Ramirez-Amaya, V., Alvarez-Borda, B., & Bermudez-Rattoni, F. (1998). Differential effects of NMDA-induced lesions into the insular cortex and amygdala on the acquisition and evocation of conditioned immunosuppression: Brain, Behavior, and Immunity Vol 12(2) Jun 1998, 149-160.
  • Randich, A. (1981). The US Preexposure Phenomenon in the Conditioned Suppression Paradigm: A Role for Conditioned Situational Stimuli: Learning and Motivation Vol 12(3) Aug 1981, 321-341.
  • Randich, A., & Haggard, D. (1983). Exposure to the unconditioned stimulus alone: Effects on retention and acquisition of conditioned suppression: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 9(2) Apr 1983, 147-159.
  • Randich, A., Jacobs, W. J., LoLordo, V. M., & Sutterer, J. R. (1978). Conditioned suppression of DRL responding: Effect of UCS intensity, schedule parameter, and schedule context: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 30(1) Feb 1978, 141-150.
  • Randich, A., & Maixner, W. (1981). Acquisition of conditioned suppression and responsivity to thermal stimulation in spontaneously hypertensive, renal hypertensive and normotensive rats: Physiology & Behavior Vol 27(4) Oct 1981, 585-590.
  • Randich, A., & Rescorla, R. A. (1981). The effects of separate presentations of the US on conditioned suppression: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 9(1) Feb 1981, 56-64.
  • Rassin, E., Diepstraten, P., Merckelbach, H., & Muris, P. (2001). Thought-action fusion and thought suppression in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Behaviour Research and Therapy Vol 39(7) 2001, 757-764.
  • Rauhut, A. S., Thomas, B. L., & Ayres, J. J. B. (2001). Treatments that weaken Pavlovian conditioned fear and thwart its renewal in rats: Implications for treating human phobias: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 27(2) Apr 2001, 99-114.
  • Rawashdeh, O., de Borsett, N. H., Roman, G., & Cahill, G. M. (2007). Melatonin suppresses nighttime memory formation in zebrafish: Science Vol 318(5853) Nov 2007, 1144-1146.
  • Rawlins, J. N., & et al. (1980). The effects of chlordiazepoxide HCL administration upon punishment and conditioned suppression in the rat: Psychopharmacology Vol 70(3) Oct 1980, 317-322.
  • Rawson, R. A., Leitenberg, H., Mulick, J. A., & Lefebvre, M. F. (1977). Recovery of extinction responding in rats following discontinuation of reinforcement of alternative behavior: A test of two explanations: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 5(4) Nov 1977, 415-420.
  • Ray, D., & Nagy, Z. M. (1978). Emerging cholinergic mechanisms and ontogeny of response inhibition in the mouse: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 92(2) Apr 1978, 335-349.
  • Ray, D. W. (1976). The role of emerging cholinergic mechanisms in the ontogeny of response inhibition in the developing mouse: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Razran, G. H. S. (1934). Conditioned withdrawal responses with shock as the conditioning stimulus in adult human subjects: Psychological Bulletin Vol 31(2) Feb 1934, 111-143.
  • Reberg, D. (1972). Compound tests for excitation in early acquisition and after prolonged extinction of conditioned suppression: Learning and Motivation Vol 3(3) Aug 1972, 246-258.
  • Reberg, D. (1978). The feature positive effect in conditioned suppression: Effects of simultaneous and sequential compound stimuli: Psychological Record Vol 28(4) Fal 1978, 595-604.
  • Reberg, D., & Leclerc, R. (1977). A feature positive effect in conditioned suppression: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 5(2) May 1977, 143-147.
  • Reberg, D., & Memmott, J. (1979). Shock as a signal for shock or no-shock: A feature-negative effect in conditioned suppression: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 32(3) Nov 1979, 387-397.
  • Reed, P. (1995). Compound stimulus preexposure effects in an appetitive conditioning procedure: Learning and Motivation Vol 26(1) Feb 1995, 1-10.
  • Reed, P. (1995). Enhanced latent inhibition following pre-exposure: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 48B(1) Feb 1995, 32-45.
  • Reiter, L. A., & deVellis, B. M. (1976). Conditioned suppression in humans produced by a human stimulus: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Vol 34(2) Aug 1976, 223-227.
  • Rescorla, R. A. (1967). Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psychological Review, 74, 71-80.
  • Rescorla, R. A. (1973). Effects of US habituation following conditioning: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 82(1) Jan 1973, 137-143.
  • Rescorla, R. A. (1976). Second-order conditioning of Pavlovian conditioned inhibition: Learning and Motivation Vol 7(2) May 1976, 161-172.
  • Rescorla, R. A. (1982). Simultaneous second-order conditioning produces S-S learning in conditioned suppression: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 8(1) Jan 1982, 23-32.
  • Rescorla, R. A. (1982). Simultaneous second-order conditioning produces Sss learning in conditioned suppression: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 8(1) Jan 1982, 23-32.
  • Rescorla, R. A. (1984). Signaling intertrial shocks attenuates their negative effect on conditioned suppression: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 22(3) May 1984, 225-228.
  • Rescorla, R. A. (1987). Facilitation and inhibition: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 13(3) Jul 1987, 250-259.
  • Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Facilitation based on inhibition: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 16(2) May 1988, 169-176.
  • Rescorla, R. A. (1989). Simultaneous and sequential conditioned inhibition in autoshaping: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 41(3-B) Aug 1989, 275-286.
  • Rescorla, R. A. (1991). Separate reinforcement can enhance the effectiveness of modulators: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 17(3) Jul 1991, 259-269.
  • Rescorla, R. A. (2002). Effect of following an excitatory-inhibitory compound with an intermediate reinforcer: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 28(2) Apr 2002, 163-174.
  • Rescorla, R. A. (2003). Protection from extinction: Learning & Behavior Vol 31(2) May 2003, 124-132.
  • Rescorla, R. A. (2004). Superconditioning from a reduced reinforcer: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 57B(2) Apr 2004, 133-152.
  • Rescorla, R. A., & Colwill, R. M. (1983). Within-compound associations in unblocking: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 9(4) Oct 1983, 390-400.
  • Rescorla, R. A., & Cunningham, C. L. (1978). Recovery of the US representation over time during extinction: Learning and Motivation Vol 9(4) Nov 1978, 373-391.
  • Rescorla, R. A., & Holland, P. C. (1977). Associations in Pavlovian conditioned inhibition: Learning and Motivation Vol 8(4) Nov 1977, 429-447.
  • Rhodes, S. E. V., & Killcross, A. S. (2007). Lesions of rat infralimbic cortex result in disrupted retardation but normal summation test performance following training on a Pavlovian conditioned inhibition procedure: European Journal of Neuroscience Vol 26(9) Nov 2007, 2654-2660.
  • Riccio, D. C., & Haroutunian, V. (1977). Failure to learn in a taste aversion paradigm: Associative or performance deficit? : Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 10(3) Sep 1977, 219-222.
  • Riccio, D. C., & Thach, J. S., Jr. (1966). Rotation as an aversive stimulus for rats: Psychonomic Science Vol 5(7) 1966, 267-268.
  • Richter, L. (1998). Suppression/hyper-activation and insufficient encoding as underlying mechanisms of motivated cognition. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Rickert, E. J., Bennett, T. L., Lane, P., & French, J. (1978). Hippocampectomy and the attenuation of blocking: Behavioral & Neural Biology Vol 22(2) Feb 1978, 147-160.
  • Rieg, T. S., Smith, N. F., & Vyse, S. A. (1993). Differential reinforcement of other behavior and response suppression: The effects of the reponse-reinforcement interval: Psychological Record Vol 43(2) Spr 1993, 271-288.
  • Riess, D., & Farrar, C. H. (1973). UCS duration and conditioned suppression: Acquisition and extinction between-groups and terminal performance within-subjects: Learning and Motivation Vol 4(4) Nov 1973, 366-373.
  • Riley, A. L., Lotter, E. C., & Kulkosky, P. J. (1979). The effects of conditioned taste aversions on the acquisition and maintenance of schedule-induced polydipsia: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 7(1) Feb 1979, 3-12.
  • Ritchie, B. G. (1997). The role of suppression in comprehension: A working memory perspective. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Roberts, A. E., Cooper, K. G., & Richey, T. L. (1977). Rat behaviors during unsignaled avoidance and conditioned suppression training: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 9(5) May 1977, 373-376.
  • Roberts, A. E., & Hurwitz, H. M. (1979). The conditioned suppression and enhancement of avoidance during a serial compound CS: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 14(1) Jul 1979, 7-10.
  • Roberts, A. E., & Porter, A. G. (1977). Conditioned suppression and enhancement of avoidance in rats: The duration of US: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 9(2) Feb 1977, 100-102.
  • Roberts, A. E., & Porter, J. W. (1976). CS-UCS training, method of baseline shock delivery, and the conditioned suppression of shuttle avoidance: Psychological Record Vol 26(3) Sum 1976, 377-386.
  • Roberts, L. E., Rau, H., Lutzenberger, W., & Birbaumer, N. (1994). Mapping P300 waves onto inhibition: Go/No-Go discrimination: Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology: Evoked Potentials Vol 92(1) Jan 1994, 44-45.
  • Roberts, R. J., Hager, L. D., & Heron, C. (1994). Prefrontal cognitive processes: Working memory and inhibition in the antisaccade task: Journal of Experimental Psychology: General Vol 123(4) Dec 1994, 374-393.
  • Robinson, G. B., Port, R. L., & Stillwell, E. J. (1993). Latent inhibition of the classically conditioned rabbit nictitating membrane response is unaffected by the NMDA antagonist MK 801: Psychobiology Vol 21(2) Jun 1993, 120-124.
  • Rodriguez, G., Alonso, G., & Lombas, A. S. (2006). Los ensayos previos que bloquean estorban aprender acerca de las agregadas C durante ensayos de condicionar de recinto con un EEUU intensificados: International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy Vol 6(3) 2006, 301-312.
  • Romaniuk, C. B., & Williams, D. A. (2000). Conditioning across the duration of a backward conditioned stimulus: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 26(4) Oct 2000, 454-461.
  • Rosellini, R. A., & DeCola, J. P. (1988). The effect of feedback stimulus duration upon contextual fear and conditioned inhibition: Learning and Motivation Vol 19(4) Nov 1988, 369-386.
  • Rudy, J. W. (1994). Ontogeny of context-specific latent inhibition of conditioned fear: Implications for configural associations theory and hippocampal formation development: Developmental Psychobiology Vol 27(6) Sep 1994, 367-379.
  • Russell, P. J. (1975). Conditioned suppression of behavior maintained by intracranial stimulation as a function of stimulation intensity: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 23(2) Mar 1975, 277-283.
  • Saari, M., & Pappas, B. A. (1973). Neonatanal 6-hydroxydopamine sympathectomy reduces foot shock-induced suppression of water-licking in normotensive and hypertensive rats: Nature-New Biology Vol 244(136) Aug 1973, 181-183.
  • Sakurai, Y., & Hirano, T. (1983). Memory confinement versus generalization decrement on retrograde amnesia produced by cortical spreading depression: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 53(6) Feb 1983, 372-376.
  • Saladin, M. E. (1993). Conditioned inhibition in sensory preconditioning: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Saladin, M. E., & Tait, R. W. (1986). US preexposures retard excitatory and facilitate inhibitory conditioning of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 14(2) May 1986, 121-132.
  • Salgado, J. V., Hetem, L. A., Vidal, M., Graeff, F. G., Danion, J. M., & Sandner, G. (2000). Reduction of latent inhibition by {d}-amphetamine in a conditioned suppression paradigm in humans: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 117(1-2) Dec 2000, 61-67.
  • Salgado, J. V., Vidal, M., Oberling, P., Graeff, F. G., Danion, J. M., & Sandner, G. (2000). Associative learning and latent inhibition in a conditioned suppression paradigm in humans: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 117(1-2) Dec 2000, 53-60.
  • Salmon, P., & Gray, J. A. (1985). Comparison between the effects of and chlordiazepoxide on timing behaviour in the rat: Psychopharmacology Vol 87(2) Oct 1985, 219-224.
  • Salmon, P., & Gray, J. A. (1986). Effects of on conditioned suppression, discriminated punishment and discriminated non-reward in the rat: Psychopharmacology Vol 88(2) Feb 1986, 252-257.
  • Sampson, L. D., Francis, J., & Schneiderman, N. (1974). Selective autonomic blockades: Effects upon classical conditioning of heart rate and lever-lift suppression in rabbits: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 87(5) Nov 1974, 953-962.
  • Sanger, D. J., & Blackman, D. E. (1976). The effects of d-amphetamine on the temporal control of operant responding in rats during a preshock stimulus: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 26(3) Nov 1976, 369-378.
  • Savastano, H. I., Arcediano, F., Stout, S. C., & Miller, R. R. (2003). Interaction between preexposure and overshadowing: Further analysis of the extended comparator hypothesis: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 56B(4) Nov 2003, 371-395.
  • Savastano, H. I., Arcediano, F., Stout, S. C., & Miller, R. R. (2007). "Interaction between preexposure and overshadowing: Further analysis of the extended comparator hypothesis": Retraction: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 59(12) Dec 2007, 2207.
  • Savastano, H. I., Cole, R. P., Barnet, R. C., & Miller, R. R. (1999). Reconsidering conditioned inhibition: Learning and Motivation Vol 30(1) Feb 1999, 101-127.
  • Schachtman, T. R. (1986). Conditioned inhibitory training of a CS paired with a pretrained inhibitor produces enhanced retardation of excitatory responding: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Schachtman, T. R., Brown, A. M., Gordon, E. L., Catterson, D. A., & Miller, R. R. (1987). Mechanisms underlying retarded emergence of conditioned responding following inhibitory training: Evidence for the comparator hypothesis: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 13(3) Jul 1987, 310-322.
  • Schachtman, T. R., Channell, S., & Hall, G. (1987). Effects of CS preexposure on inhibition of delay: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 15(3) Aug 1987, 301-311.
  • Schachtman, T. R., Gee, J.-L., Kasprow, W. J., & Miller, R. R. (1983). Reminder-induced recovery from blocking as a function of the number of compound trials: Learning and Motivation Vol 14(2) May 1983, 154-164.
  • Schachtman, T. R., Kasprow, W. J., & Miller, R. R. (1984). Reminder treatments do not alleviate cue-to-consequence deficits: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 12(1) Feb 1984, 97-105.
  • Schachtman, T. R., Matzel, L. D., & Miller, R. R. (1988). Retardation of conditioned excitation following operational inhibitory blocking: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 16(1) Feb 1988, 100-104.
  • Schachtman, T. R., Threlkeld, R., & Meyer, K. (2000). Retention of conditioned inhibition produced by extinction: Learning and Motivation Vol 31(4) Nov 2000, 283-300.
  • Schindler, C. W., Thorndike, E. B., Ma, J. D., & Goldberg, S. R. (2000). Conditioned suppression with cocaine as the unconditioned stimulus: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 65(1) Jan 2000, 83-89.
  • Schmajuk, N. A., Lam, Y.-W., & Gray, J. A. (1996). Latent inhibition: A neural network approach: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 22(3) Jul 1996, 321-349.
  • Schmidt, E., McCaleb, M., & Merrill, H. K. (1977). Food and intracranial stimulation responding suppressed with regular-interval shock: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 27(1) Jan 1977, 161-170.
  • Schmuck, P., & Bloem, R. (1998). Negative-priming-designs: Comparison of different approaches for measuring cognitive inhibition: Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle Psychologie Vol 45(1) 1998, 60-71.
  • Schneider, A. M., Weinberg, J., & Weissberg, R. (1974). Effects of ACTH on conditioned suppression: A time and strength of conditioning analysis: Physiology & Behavior Vol 13(5) Nov 1974, 633-636.
  • Schnur, P. (1975). The number of trials of compound conditioning: Its effect on latent inhibition in rats: American Journal of Psychology Vol 88(3) Sep 1975, 411-419.
  • Schonfeld, S., Ehlers, A., Bollinghaus, I., & Rief, W. (2007). Overgeneral memory and suppression of trauma memories in post-traumatic stress disorder: Memory Vol 15(3) Apr 2007, 339-352.
  • Schucker, R. E., Stevens, L. B., & Ellis, D. S. (1953). A retest for conditioned inhibition in the alphabet-printing task: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 46(2) Aug 1953, 97-102.
  • Schulteis, G., Morse, A. C., & Liu, J. (2004). Conditioning processes contribute to severity of naloxone-precipitated withdrawal from acute opioid dependence: Psychopharmacology Vol 175(4) Oct 2004, 463-472.
  • Schulze, G. E., Benson, R. W., Paule, M. G., & Roberts, D. W. (1988). Behaviorally conditioned suppression of murine T-cell dependent but not T-cell independent antibody responses: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 30(4) Aug 1988, 859-865.
  • Schwartz, B. (1976). Positive and negative conditioned suppression in the pigeon: Effects of the locus and modality of the CS: Learning and Motivation Vol 7(1) Feb 1976, 86-100.
  • Schweitzer, L., & Green, L. (1982). Reevaluation of things past: A test of the "retrospection hypothesis" using a CER procedure with rats: Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science Vol 17(2) Apr-Jun 1982, 62-68.
  • Scobie, S. R. (1973). The response-shock interval and conditioned suppression of avoidance in rats: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 1(1) Feb 1973, 17-20.
  • Seger, K. A., & Scheuer, C. (1977). The informational properties of S1, S2, and the S1-S2 sequence on conditioned suppression: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 5(1) Feb 1977, 39-41.
  • Senter, R. J., & Hummel, W. F. (1965). Suppression of an autonomic response through operant conditioning: Psychological Record 15(1) 1965, 1-5.
  • Shephard, R. A., Toal, L., & Leslie, J. C. (1990). Effects of agonists and antagonists at the GABA/benzodiazepine receptor on conditioned suppression in rats: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 36(1) May 1990, 39-43.
  • Sherman, J. E. (1978). Simultaneous fear conditioning: A comparison with forward procedures: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Sherman, J. E. (1978). US inflation with trace and simultaneous fear conditioning: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 6(4) Nov 1978, 463-468.
  • Sherman, J. E., & Maier, S. F. (1978). The decrement in conditioned fear with increased trials of simultaneous conditioning is not specific to the simultaneous procedure: Learning and Motivation Vol 9(1) Feb 1978, 31-53.
  • Shevill, I., & Hall, G. (2004). Retrospective revaluation effects in the conditioned suppression procedure: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 57B(4) Oct 2004, 331-347.
  • Shibata, K., Kataoka, Y., Yamashita, K., & Ueki, S. (1986). An important role of the central amygdaloid nucleus and mammillary body in the mediation of conflict behavior in rats: Brain Research Vol 372(1) Apr 1986, 159-162.
  • Shipley, R. H. (1973). Extinction of conditioned suppression of licking as a function of CS duration in acquisition, CS duration in extinction, and total nonreinforced CS exposure: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Shul'gina, G. I. (1998). Effect of sodium nitrite on the execution of defensive and inhibitory conditioned reflexes: Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology Vol 28(3) May-Jun 1998, 278-284.
  • Shulgina, G. I., & Pavlova, I. V. (1982). The role of cholinergic transmission of inhibitory and disinhibiting influences in the mechanisms of reinforcement and internal inhibition: Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel'nosti Vol 32(4) Jul-Aug 1982, 616-625.
  • Shul'gina, G. I., & Voronina, G. A. (1997). Effects of dilantin on the inhibition of neocortical and hippocampal neurons under conditions of the interaction of a nonrelevant stimulus and the dominant state of a defensive reflex: Human Physiology Vol 23(4) Jul-Aug 1997, 423-428.
  • Siegel, R. A. (1978). Probability of punishment and suppression of behavior in psychopathic and nonpsychopathic offenders: Journal of Abnormal Psychology Vol 87(5) Oct 1978, 514-522.
  • Sipes, T. A., & Geyer, M. A. (1994). Multiple serotonin receptor subtypes modulate prepulse inhibition of the startle response in rats: Neuropharmacology Vol 33(3-4) Mar-Apr 1994, 441-448.
  • Siviy, S. M., & Harrison, K. A. (2008). Effects of neonatal handling on play behavior and fear towards a predator odor in juvenile rats (rattus norvegicus): Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 122(1) Feb 2008, 1-8.
  • Skinner, N. F. (1974). Comment on Sachs and May's paper on the conditioned emotional response with humans: Psychological Reports Vol 34(3, Pt 2) Jun 1974, 1069-1070.
  • Slifer, K. J., Cataldo, M. F., Cataldo, M. D., Llorente, A. M., & et al. (1993). Behavior analysis of motion control for pediatric neuroimaging: Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis Vol 26(4) Win 1993, 469-470.
  • Smith, B. H. (1997). An analysis of blocking in odorant mixtures: An increase but not a decrease in intensity of reinforcement produces unblocking: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 111(1) Feb 1997, 57-69.
  • Smith, J. M., Renault, P. F., & Schuster, C. R. (1973). Operant schedule control of differential changes in gastrointestinal motility: Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association 1973, 855-856.
  • Smith, M. A., Bair, W., & Movshon, J. A. (2006). Dynamics of Suppression in Macaque Primary Visual Cortex: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 26(18) May 2006, 4826-4834.
  • Solomon, P. R. (1977). Role of the hippocampus in blocking and conditioned inhibition of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 91(2) Apr 1977, 407-417.
  • Spencer, L. T. (1923). Central Inhibition in the Albino Rat: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 3(5) Oct 1923, 389-408.
  • Spevack, A. A., Campbell, C. T., & Drake, L. (1975). Effect of amygdalectomy on habituation and CER in rats: Physiology & Behavior Vol 15(2) Aug 1975, 199-207.
  • Spiker, C. C., & Lubker, B. J. (1964). Experimental tests of the hypothesis of stimulus interaction: Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 1(3) 1964, 256-268.
  • St. Claire-Smith, R., & Mackintosh, N. J. (1974). Complete suppression to a compound CS does not block further conditioning to each element: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 28(1) Mar 1974, 92-101.
  • St. Claire-Smith, R. J. (1979). The overshadowing and blocking of punishment: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 31(1) Feb 1979, 51-61.
  • Starkweather, J. A., & Duncan, C. P. (1954). A test for conditioned inhibition in motor learning: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 47(5) May 1954, 351-356.
  • Stephens, D. N., & Tonkiss, J. (1981). Rats undernourished as infants differ from controls in learning about a compound but not a simple discriminative stimulus: Physiology & Behavior Vol 26(5) May 1981, 803-808.
  • Steranka, L. R., & Barrett, R. J. (1974). Facilitation of avoidance acquisition by lesion of the median raphe nucleus: Evidence for serotonin as a mediator of shock-induced suppression: Behavioral Biology Vol 11(2) Jun 1974, 205-213.
  • Stewart, R. E., Heck, G. L., & Desimone, J. A. (1996). Taste-mixture suppression: Functional dissection of cellular and paracellular origins: Journal of Neurophysiology Vol 75(5) May 1996, 2124-2128.
  • Stitzer, M. (1974). Comparison of morphine and chlorpromazine effects on moderately and severely suppressed punished responding in the pigeon: Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics Vol 191(1) Oct 1974, 172-178.
  • Stoffer, G. R., & Babb, H. (1976). Transfer suppression of a hunger-motivated response as a function of the number of prior escape or avoidance trials: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 7(5) May 1976, 471-474.
  • Stout, S., Arcediano, F., Escobar, M., & Miller, R. R. (2003). Overshadowing as a function of trial number: Dynamics of first- and second-order comparator effects: Learning & Behavior Vol 31(1) Feb 2003, 85-97.
  • Stout, S., Escobar, M., & Miller, R. R. (2004). Trial number and compound stimuli temporal relationship as joint determinants of second-order conditioning and conditioned inhibition: Learning & Behavior Vol 32(2) May 2004, 230-239.
  • Stout, S. C., Chang, R., & Miller, R. R. (2003). Trial spacing is a determinant of cue interaction: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 29(1) Jan 2003, 23-38.
  • Stowell, J. R., Berntson, G. G., & Sarter, M. (2000). Attenuation of the bidirectional effects of chlordiazepoxide and FG 7142 on conditioned response suppression and associated cardiovascular reactivity by loss of cortical cholinergic inputs: Psychopharmacology Vol 150(2) Jun 2000, 141-149.
  • Stromberg, M. F., Volpicelli, J. R., & O'Brien, C. P. (1998). Effects of naltrexone administered repeatedly across 30 or 60 days on ethanol consumption using a limited access procedure in the rat: Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research Vol 22(9) Dec 1998, 2186-2191.
  • Stubbs, D. A., Hughes, J. E., & Cohen, S. L. (1978). Positive conditioned suppression: An explanation in terms of multiple and concurrent schedules: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 30(3) Nov 1978, 329-343.
  • Sturdy, C. B., Phillmore, L. S., & Weisman, R. G. (2000). Call-note discriminations in black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus): Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 114(4) Dec 2000, 357-364.
  • Suiping, W., Yukai, C., Ruiming, W., & Yan, W. (2005). The Activation and Suppression of Different Anaphoric Referents during Discourse Comprehension: Psychological Science (China) Vol 28(2) Mar 2005, 342-346.
  • Sutton, R. L., Tomlinson, W., & Hicks, R. A. (1982). Preacquisition REM sleep deprivation inhibits the formation of conditioned feeding suppression in rats: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 20(6) Dec 1982, 308-310.
  • Swartzentruber, D., & Bouton, M. E. (1986). Contextual control of negative transfer produced by prior CS-US pairings: Learning and Motivation Vol 17(4) Nov 1986, 366-385.
  • Swartzentruber, D., & Bouton, M. E. (1992). Context sensitivity of conditioned suppression following preexposure to the conditioned stimulus: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 20(2) May 1992, 97-103.
  • Swartzentruber, D., & Rescorla, R. A. (1994). Modulation of trained and extinguished stimuli by facilitators and inhibitors: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 22(3) Aug 1994, 309-316.
  • Swartzwelder, H. S. (1981). The use of a previously poisoned taste as a reinforcer and discriminative stimulus for operant licking: Eliciting and cueing properties: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Symonds, M., & Hall, G. (1995). Perceptual learning in flavor aversion conditioning: Roles of stimulus comparison and latent inhibition of common stimulus elements: Learning and Motivation Vol 26(2) May 1995, 203-219.
  • Symonds, M., & Hall, G. (1997). Contextual conditioning with lithium-induced nausea as the US: Evidence from a blocking procedure: Learning and Motivation Vol 28(2) May 1997, 200-215.
  • Tai, C. T., Cassaday, H. J., Feldon, J., & Rawlins, J. N. P. (1995). Both electrolytic and excitotoxic lesions of nucleus accumbens disrupt latent inhibition of learning in rats: Neurobiology of Learning and Memory Vol 64(1) Jul 1995, 36-48.
  • Tailby, C., Solomon, S. G., Peirce, J. W., & Metha, A. B. (2007). Two expressions of "surround suppression" in V1 that arise independent of cortical mechanisms of suppression: Visual Neuroscience Vol 24(1) Jan-Feb 2007, 99-109.
  • Tait, R. W., & Saladin, M. E. (1986). Concurrent development of excitatory and inhibitory associations during backward conditioning: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 14(2) May 1986, 133-137.
  • Takahashi, L. K., & Kim, H. (1995). Relative contributions of pituitary-adrenal hormones to the ontogeny of behavioral inhibition in the rat: Physiology & Behavior Vol 57(4) Apr 1995, 711-716.
  • Takeuchi, T., Hayashida, K.-i., Inagaki, H., Kuwahara, M., Tsubone, H., & Harada, E. (2003). Opioid mediated suppressive effect of milk-derived lactoferrin on distress induced by maternal separation in rat pups: Brain Research Vol 979(1-2) Jul 2003, 216-224.
  • Tapp, P. D., Siwak, C. T., Estrada, J., Head, E., Muggenburg, B. A., Cotman, C. W., et al. (2003). Size and reversal learning in the beagle dog as a measure of executive function and inhibitory control in aging: Learning & Memory Vol 10(1) Jan-Feb 2003, 64-73.
  • Teghtsoonian, R., & Shepherd, A. H. (1957). Reactive and conditioned inhibition in perceptual-motor performance: Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Vol 11(4) Dec 1957, 201-211.
  • Telford, C. W., & Anderson, B. O. (1932). The normal wink reflex; its facilitation and inhibition: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 15(3) Jun 1932, 235-266.
  • Terry, P., & Salmon, P. (1991). Anxiolytic-like action of beta-blockers: Effects of stimulus salience: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 39(3) Jul 1991, 597-603.
  • Terry, W. S. (1985). The inhibitory effect of pretrial reinforcement on instrumental conditioning: Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science Vol 20(2) Apr-Jun 1985, 78-83.
  • Tessari, A., & Rumiati, R. I. (2002). Motor distal component and pragmatic representation of objects: Cognitive Brain Research Vol 14(2) Aug 2002, 218-227.
  • Testa, T. J. (1975). Effects of similarity of location and temporal intensity pattern of conditioned and unconditioned stimuli on the acquisition of conditioned suppression in rats: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 1(2) Apr 1975, 114-121.
  • Thaw, A. K., & Smith, J. C. (1992). Conditioned suppression as a method of detecting taste thresholds in the rat: Chemical Senses Vol 17(2) Apr 1992, 211-223.
  • Thiebot, M. H., Laporte, A. M., & Soubrie, P. (1987). Microgram doses of diazepam do not induce proconvulsant or proconflict effects in rodents: Psychopharmacology Vol 93(3) Nov 1987, 389-392.
  • Thiebot, M.-H., Hamon, M., & Soubrie, P. (1983). The involvement of nigral serotonin innervation in the control of punishment-induced behavioral inhibition in rats: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 19(2) Aug 1983, 225-229.
  • Thomas, B. L., & Ayres, J. J. B. (2004). Use of the ABA fear renewal paradigm to assess the effects of extinction with co-present fear inhibitors or excitors: Implications for theories of extinction and for treating human fears and phobias: Learning and Motivation Vol 35(1) Feb 2004, 22-52.
  • Thomas, B. L., Larsen, N., & Ayres, J. J. B. (2003). Role of context similarity in ABA, ABC, and AAB renewal paradigms: Implications for theories of renewal and for treating human phobias: Learning and Motivation Vol 34(4) Nov 2003, 410-436.
  • Thomas, B. L., Longo, C. L., & Ayres, J. J. B. (2005). Thwarting the renewal (relapse) of conditioned fear with the explicitly unpaired procedure: Possible interpretations and implications for treating human fears and phobias: Learning and Motivation Vol 36(4) Nov 2005, 374-407.
  • Thomas, D. A. (1978). Retention of excitation and inhibition in a Pavlovian fear conditioning paradigm: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Thomas, D. A. (1979). Retention of conditioned inhibition in a bar-press suppression paradigm: Learning and Motivation Vol 10(2) May 1979, 161-177.
  • Thomas, D. A., Haroutunian, V., & Barfield, R. J. (1981). Behavioral and physiological habituation to an ultrasonic stimulus: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 17(6) Jun 1981, 279-282.
  • Thomas, D. A., & Riccio, D. C. (1979). Forgetting of a CS attribute in a conditioned suppression paradigm: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 7(2) May 1979, 191-195.
  • Thompson, J. B., & Schwartzbaum, J. S. (1964). Discrimination behavior and conditioned suppression (CER) following localized lesions in the amygdala and putamen: Psychological Reports 15(2) 1964, 587-606.
  • Thorne, B. M., McDougal, Y., & Topping, J. S. (1976). Olfactory bulb removal and response suppression in rats: Physiology & Behavior Vol 17(2) Aug 1976, 259-265.
  • Tierney, K. J., Smith, H. V., & Gannon, K. N. (1983). Effects of switching rate and changeover requirement on performance on nondepriving schedules: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 9(3) Jul 1983, 281-291.
  • Tinsley, M. R., Timberlake, W., Sitomer, M., & Widman, D. R. (2002). Conditioned inhibitory effects of discriminated Pavlovian training with food in rats depend on interactions of search modes, related repertoires, and response measures: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 30(3) Aug 2002, 217-227.
  • Toal, L., Leslie, J. C., & Shephard, R. A. (1991). Effects of chlordiazepoxide and putative anxiogenics on conditioned suppression in rats: Physiology & Behavior Vol 49(6) Jun 1991, 1085-1090.
  • Tobler, P. N., Dickinson, A., & Schultz, W. (2003). Coding of Predicted Reward Omission by Dopamine Neurons in a Conditioned Inhibition Paradigm: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 23(32) Nov 2003, 10402-10410.
  • Tomaz, C., & Huston, J. P. (1986). Facilitation of conditioned inhibitory avoidance by post-trial peripheral injection of substance P: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 25(2) Aug 1986, 469-472.
  • Topping, J. S., Surratt, C. M., & Barrios, B. A. (1977). Relative effects of omission training and extinction training in obese subjects: Psychological Record Vol 27(2) Spr 1977, 449-456.
  • Topping, J. S., Thompson, H. J., & Barrios, B. A. (1976). Comparison of omission training and extinction training in mentally retarded individuals: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 8(3) Sep 1976, 211-214.
  • Trego, L. L. (2008). Menstruation during deployment: Military women's attitudes towards menstrual suppression. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Tsaltas, E., & et al. (1984). Dorsal bundle lesions do not affect latent inhibition of conditioned suppression: Psychopharmacology Vol 84(4) Dec 1984, 549-555.
  • Tsaltas, E., Gray, J. A., & Fillenz, M. (1984). Alleviation of response suppression to conditioned aversive stimuli by lesions of the dorsal noradrenergic bundle: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 13(2) Aug 1984, 115-127.
  • Tsaltas, E., Schugens, M. M., & Gray, J. A. (1989). Effects of lesions of the dorsal noradrenergic bundle on conditioned suppression to a CS and to a contextual background stimulus: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 31(3) Jan 1989, 243-256.
  • Tsuda, Y. (1983). The effects of posttrial episodes on conditioning in rats: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 54(4) Oct 1983, 268-272.
  • Turner, E. G. (1976). Conditioned suppression of an operant response using drugs as conditioned stimuli: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Turner, E. G., & Altshuler, H. L. (1976). Conditioned suppression of an operant response using d -amphetamine as the conditioned stimulus: Psychopharmacology Vol 50(2) 1976, 139-143.
  • Uchida, M. (1984). The effects of discrimination of punishment on conditioned suppression in rats: Japanese Journal of Psychonomic Science Vol 3(2) Dec 1984, 101-107.
  • Uchida, M. (1991). The effects of response-electric shock contigency upon the conditioned suppression of rats in discriminative punishment: Japanese Journal of Psychonomic Science Vol 9(2) Mar 1991, 95-104.
  • Uegaki, H. (1976). The effects of pretraining on compound conditioning: Sutherland-Mackintosh's selective attention hypothesis or Rescorla-Wagner's simple S-R classical conditioning hypothesis? : Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 47(2) Jun 1976, 85-91.
  • Urcelay, G. P., & Miller, R. R. (2006). A Comparator View of Pavlovian and Differential Inhibition: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 32(3) Jul 2006, 271-283.
  • Urushihara, K. (2000). Classical backward conditioning in second-order conditioning in rat's conditioned suppression: Japanese Journal of Animal Psychology Vol 50(1) Jun 2000, 27-31.
  • Urushihara, K., & Imada, H. (1999). Effect of temporal relationship between first-order conditioned stimulus and unconditioned stimulus on classical second-order conditioning in rats: Japanese Journal of Animal Psychology Vol 49(2) Dec 1999, 161-170.
  • Urushihara, K., Wheeler, D. S., & Miller, R. R. (2004). Outcome Pre- and Postexposure Effects: Retention Interval Interacts With Primacy and Recency: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 30(4) Oct 2004, 283-298.
  • Valentine, J. O., & Barrett, J. E. (1981). Effects of chlordiazepoxide and d-amphetamine on responding suppressed by conditioned punishment: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior Vol 35(2) Mar 1981, 209-216.
  • van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., van Boxtel, G. J. M., & van der Molen, M. W. (2003). The duration of response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm varies with response force: Acta Psychologica Vol 114(2) Oct 2003, 115-129.
  • van Erp, A. M. M., & Miczek, K. A. (2001). Persistent suppression of ethanol self-administration by brief social stress in rats and increased startle response as index of withdrawal: Physiology & Behavior Vol 73(3) Jun 2001, 301-311.
  • van Haaren, F., & Anderson, K. G. (1997). Effects of chlordiazepoxide, buspirone and cocaine on behavior suppressed by timeout presentation: Behavioural Pharmacology Vol 8(2-3) Jun 1997, 174-182.
  • van Swinderen, B., & Hall, J. C. (1995). Analysis of conditioned courtship in dusky: Andante rhythm mutants of Drosophila: Learning & Memory Vol 2(2) Mar-Apr 1995, 49-61.
  • Van Willigen, F., Emmett, J., Cote, D., & Ayres, J. J. (1987). CS modality effects in one-trial backward and forward excitatory conditioning as assessed by conditioned suppression of licking in rats: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 15(2) May 1987, 201-211.
  • Van Willigen, F., Emmett, J., Cote, D., & Ayres, J. J. (1989). "CS modality effects in one-trial backward and forward excitatory conditioning as assessed by conditioned suppression of licking in rats": Erratum: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 17(4) Nov 1989, 481.
  • Vanman, E. J., Boehmelt, A. H., Dawson, M. E., & Schell, A. M. (1996). The varying time courses of attentional and affective modulation of the startle eyeblink reflex: Psychophysiology Vol 33(6) Nov 1996, 691-697.
  • Vaysse, G., Galissie, M., & Corbiere, M. (1988). Induced variation of serotonin in Drosophila melanogaster and its relation to learning performance: Journal of Comparative Psychology Vol 102(3) Sep 1988, 225-229.
  • Vetulani, J., Sansone, M., Baran, L., & Hano, J. (1984). Opposite action of m-chlorophenylpiperazine on avoidance depression induced by trazodone and pimozide in CD-1 mice: Psychopharmacology Vol 83(2) May 1984, 166-168.
  • Vigorito, M., & Ayres, J. J. (1987). Effect of naloxone on conditioned suppression in rats: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 101(4) Aug 1987, 576-586.
  • Vila Carranza, J. (1980). Brief notes on conditioned inhibition: Revista Mexicana de Analisis de la Conducta Vol 6(2) Dec 1980, 201-209.
  • Vouimba, R.-M., Garcia, R., Baudry, M., & Thompson, R. F. (2000). Potentiation of conditioned freezing following dorsomedial prefrontal cortex lesions does not interfere with fear reduction in mice: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 114(4) Aug 2000, 720-724.
  • Walasek, G., Wesierska, M., & Werka, T. (2002). Effects of social rearing conditions on conditioned suppression in rats: Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis Vol 62(1) 2002, 25-31.
  • Walasek, G., & Zielinski, K. (1987). Training of conditioned enhancement and conditioned suppression in rats: Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis Vol 47(1) 1987, 1-18.
  • Walasek, G., & Zielinski, K. (1989). Stimulus modality effects on transformations of conditioned enhancement and conditioned suppression in rats: Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis 49(5) 1989, 215-238.
  • Walker, D. J., & Branch, M. N. (1998). Response suppression during cumulative dosing: A role for Pavlovian conditioning: Behavioural Pharmacology Vol 9(3) May 1998, 255-271.
  • Walker, L. C., Popovich, B. G., & Derby, T. R. (1973). Intermittent and continuous punishment of operant and consummatory behaviors of albino rats: Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association 1973, 859-860.
  • Wallace, M. D. (2001). Mechanisms of response suppression under noncontingent reinforcement. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering.
  • Walters, G. C. (1978). Amount of prior learning, density of reinforcement and vacation from punishment as determinants of punishment effectiveness: Some negative results: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 11(1) Jan 1978, 33-36.
  • Walters, G. C., & Herring, B. (1978). Differential suppression by punishment of nonconsummatory licking and lever pressing: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 4(2) Apr 1978, 170-187.
  • Walton, D. (1963). The effects of drive (D), reactive (I-sub(R)) and conditioned inhibition (-sub(S)I-sub(R)): Their application to the remedial treatment of an adolescent psychopath: Behaviour Research and Therapy 1(1) 1963, 35-43.
  • Warburton, E. C., Joseph, M. H., Feldon, J., Weiner, I., & et al. (1994). Antagonism of amphetamine-induced disruption of latent inhibition in rats by haloperidol and ondansetron: Implications for a possible antipsychotic action of ondansetron: Psychopharmacology Vol 114(4) May 1994, 657-664.
  • Watanabe, S., & Ono, K. (1986). An experimental analysis of "empathic" response: Effects of pain reactions of pigeon upon other pigeon's operant behavior: Behavioural Processes Vol 13(3) Sep 1986, 269-277.
  • Watson, J. B. (1916). The place of the conditioned-reflex in psychology: Psychological Review Vol 23(2) Mar 1916, 89-116.
  • Webb, B. S., Dhruv, N. T., Solomon, S. G., Tailby, C., & Lennie, P. (2005). Early and Late Mechanisms of Surround Suppression in Striate Cortex of Macaque: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 25(50) Dec 2005, 11666-11675.
  • Webbe, F. M. (1975). Maintenance and suppression of responding under concurrent schedules of electric-shock presentation: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Weiner, I., & Feldon, J. (1992). Phencyclidine does not disrupt latent inhibition in rats: Implications for animal models of schizophrenia: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 42(4) Aug 1992, 625-631.
  • Weiner, I., & Feldon, J. (1994). The latent inhibition model of schizophrenic attention disorder and of antipsychotic drug action: Comment on Dunn, Atwater and Kilts: Psychopharmacology Vol 116(3) Nov 1994, 379-380.
  • Weiner, I., Feldon, J., & Katz, Y. (1987). Facilitation of the expression but not the acquisition of latent inhibition by haloperidol in rats: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 26(2) Feb 1987, 241-246.
  • Weiner, I., Feldon, J., & Ziv-Harris, D. (1987). Early handling and latent inhibition in the conditioned suppression paradigm: Developmental Psychobiology Vol 20(2) Mar 1987, 233-240.
  • Weiner, I., Gal, G., Rawlins, J. N. P., & Feldon, J. (1996). Differential involvement of the shell and core subterritories of the nucleus accumbens in latent inhibition and amphetamine-induced activity: Behavioural Brain Research Vol 81(1-2) Dec 1996, 123-134.
  • Weiner, I., Izraeli-Telerant, A., & Feldon, J. (1987). Latent inhibition is not affected by acute or chronic administration of 6 mg/kg dl-amphetamine: Psychopharmacology Vol 91(3) Mar 1987, 345-351.
  • Weiner, I., Lubow, R. E., & Feldon, J. (1984). Abolition of the expression but not the acquisition of latent inhibition by chronic amphetamine in rats: Psychopharmacology Vol 83(2) May 1984, 194-199.
  • Weiner, I., Lubow, R. E., & Feldon, J. (1988). Disruption of latent inhibition by acute administration of low doses of amphetamine: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 30(4) Aug 1988, 871-878.
  • Weiner, I., Traub, A., Rawlins, J. N. P., Smith, A. D., & et al. (1995). The sigma ligand BMY-14802 as a potential antipsychotic: Evidence from the latent inhibition model in rats: Behavioural Pharmacology Vol 6(1) Jan 1995, 46-54.
  • Weisman, R. N. (1977). Sex differences in consummatory and shock determined behavior after ventromedial hypothalamic lesions: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Weldin, G. H. (1976). Artifacts in paradoxical enhancement of conditioned fear: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Welker, R. L., & Wheatley, K. L. (1977). Differential acquisition of conditioned suppression in rats with increased and decreased luminance levels as CS+s: Learning and Motivation Vol 8(2) May 1977, 247-262.
  • Welner, S. A., Rousse, I., & Rochford, J. (1995). Transplants to the cerebral cortex of nucleus basalis magnocellularis-lesioned rats: Effects on deficits in latent inhibition: Brain Research Vol 671(2) Feb 1995, 309-313.
  • Wendt, G. R. (1936). An interpretation of inhibition of conditioned reflexes as competition between reaction systems: Psychological Review Vol 43(3) May 1936, 258-281.
  • Wenjuan, L., Jihuan, C., & Husband, A. (2002). Conditioned immunosuppression to rabbit anti-rat lymphocyte serum in rats: Acta Psychologica Sinica Vol 34(3) May 2002, 295-300.
  • Wesierska, M., Brennan, J. F., & Zielinski, K. (1986). Generalization of a conditioned inhibitor of response suppression in rats: Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis Vol 46(1) 1986, 11-26.
  • Wesierska, M., & Zielinski, K. (1985). Extinction, reinstatement and spontaneous recovery of conditioned suppression in rats and the stability of conditioned inhibitor performance: Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis Vol 45(1-2) 1985, 1-24.
  • Wessells, M. G. (1973). Errorless discrimination, autoshaping, and conditioned inhibition: Science Vol 182(4115) Nov 1973, 941-943.
  • Wheeler, D. S., Chang, R. C., & Miller, R. R. (2003). Massive preexposure and preexposure in multiple contexts attenuate the context specificity of latent inhibition: Learning & Behavior Vol 31(4) Nov 2003, 378-386.
  • Wheeler, D. S., & Miller, R. R. (2007). Interactions between retroactive-interference and context-mediated treatments that impair Pavlovian conditioned responding: Learning & Behavior Vol 35(1) Feb 2007, 27-35.
  • Wheeler, D. S., Stout, S. C., & Miller, R. R. (2004). Interaction of retention interval with CS-preexposure and extinction treatments: Symmetry with respect to primacy: Learning & Behavior Vol 32(3) Aug 2004, 335-347.
  • Wheeler, D. S., Stout, S. C., & Miller, R. R. (2006). Addendum to Wheeler, Stout, and Miller (2004): Learning & Behavior Vol 34(1) Feb 2006, 109.
  • Whetstone, T., & Cross, M. D. (1998). Control of conscious contents in directed forgetting and thought suppression: Psyche: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on Consciousness Vol 4(16) Nov 1998, No Pagination Specified.
  • White, P. M., Kanazawa, A., & Yee, C. M. (2005). Gender and suppression of mid-latency ERP components during stress: Psychophysiology Vol 42(6) Nov 2005, 720-725.
  • Wickens, D. D. (1938). The transference of conditioned excitation and conditioned inhibition from one muscle group to the antagonistic muscle group: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 22(2) Feb 1938, 101-123.
  • Wickens, D. D. (1939). The simultaneous transfer of conditioned excitation and conditioned inhibition: Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 24(3) Mar 1939, 332-338.
  • Wiertelak, E. P., Watkins, L. R., & Maier, S. F. (1992). Conditioned inhibition of analgesia: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 20(4) Nov 1992, 339-349.
  • Wilcott, R. C. (1981). Medial and orbital cortex and the suppression of behavior in the rat: Physiology & Behavior Vol 27(2) Aug 1981, 237-241.
  • Wilcott, R. C. (1988). A further analysis of the response suppression during stimulation of the septal area in the rat: Physiology & Behavior Vol 42(2) 1988, 167-172.
  • Wiley, J. L., Compton, A. D., & Golden, K. M. (2000). Separation of drug effects on timing and behavioral inhibition by increased stimulus control: Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology Vol 8(4) Nov 2000, 451-461.
  • Wilkinson, L. S., Killcross, S. S., Humby, T., Hall, F. S., & et al. (1994). Social isolation in the rat produces developmentally specific deficits in prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle response without disrupting latent inhibition: Neuropsychopharmacology Vol 10(1) Feb 1994, 61-72.
  • Williams, B. A. (1994). Blocking despite changes in reinforcer identity: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 22(4) Nov 1994, 442-457.
  • Williams, D. A. (1988). Differences in the associative structure among five types of conditioned inhibitors: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Williams, D. A. (1995). Forms of inhibition in animal and human learning: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 21(2) Apr 1995, 129-142.
  • Williams, D. A., & Hurlburt, J. L. (2000). Mechanisms of second-order conditioning with a backward conditioned stimulus: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 26(3) Jul 2000, 340-351.
  • Williams, D. A., Johns, K. W., & Norton, G. R. (1998). Conditioned inhibition and its applications in panic and obsessive-compulsive disorders. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
  • Williams, D. A., LoLordo, V. M., & Overmier, J. B. (1990). Signaling the unconditioned stimulus and backward conditioned inhibition: Prior conditioning of the signal and context: Learning and Motivation Vol 21(1) Feb 1990, 32-58.
  • Williams, D. A., & Overmier, J. B. (1988). Backward inhibitory conditioning with signaled and unsignaled unconditioned stimuli: Distribution of trials across days and intertrial interval: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 14(1) Jan 1988, 26-35.
  • Williams, D. A., & Overmier, J. B. (1988). Some types of conditioned inhibitors carry collateral excitatory associations: Learning and Motivation Vol 19(4) Nov 1988, 345-368.
  • Williams, D. A., & Overmier, J. B. (1990). Nonassociative habituation, US preexposure, and backward inhibitory conditioning with signaled and unsignaled USs: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 18(1) Feb 1990, 35-43.
  • Williams, D. A., Overmier, J. B., & LoLordo, V. M. (1992). A reevaluation of Rescorla's early dictums about Pavlovian conditioned inhibition: Psychological Bulletin Vol 111(2) Mar 1992, 275-290.
  • Williams, D. A., & Sly, C. A. (1999). Recovery of conditioned suppression after backward pairings: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 27(2) May 1999, 152-167.
  • Williams, D. A., Travis, G. M., & Overmier, J. B. (1986). Within-compound associations modulate the relative effectiveness of differential and Pavlovian conditioned inhibition procedures: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 12(4) Oct 1986, 351-362.
  • Willis, R. D., & Lundin, R. W. (1966). Conditioned suppression in the rat as a function of shock reinforcement schedule: Psychonomic Science Vol 6(3) 1966, 107-108.
  • Willott, J. F., & Carlson, S. (1995). Modification of the acoustic startle response in hearing-impaired C57BL/6J mice: Prepulse augmentation and prolongation of prepulse inhibition: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 109(3) Jun 1995, 396-403.
  • Willott, J. F., Carlson, S., & Chen, H. (1994). Prepulse inhibition of the startle response in mice: Relationship to hearing loss and auditory system plasticity: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 108(4) Aug 1994, 703-713.
  • Wilson, G. K. (1974). A re-examination of the role of perceptual contrast in CS intensity dynamism: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Wilson, L. M., & Riccio, D. C. (1973). CS familiarization and conditioned suppression in weanling and adult albino rats: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 1(3) Mar 1973, 184-186.
  • Windholz, G. (1996). Hypnosis and inhibition as viewed by Heidenhain and Pavlov: Integrative Physiological & Behavioral Science Vol 31(2) Apr-Jun 1996, 155-162.
  • Winograd, E. (1965). Maintained generalization testing of conditioned suppression: Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 8(1) 1965, 47-51.
  • Witcher, E. S. (1978). Extinction of Pavlovian conditioned inhibition: Dissertation Abstracts International.
  • Witcher, E. S., & Ayres, J. J. (1975). Effect of removing background white noise during CS presentation on conditioning in the truly random control procedure: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 6(1) Jul 1975, 25-27.
  • Witcher, E. S., & Ayres, J. J. (1984). A test of two methods for extinguishing Pavlovian conditioned inhibition: Animal Learning & Behavior Vol 12(2) May 1984, 149-156.
  • Witnauer, J. E., & Miller, R. R. (2007). Degraded contingency revisited: Posttraining extinction of a cover stimulus attenuates a target cue's behavioral control: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 33(4) Oct 2007, 440-450.
  • Wittman, T. K., & DeVietti, T. L. (1981). Latent inhibition measured by heart rate suppression in rats: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 17(6) Jun 1981, 283-285.
  • Wojtczak-Jaroszowa, J. (1964). Conditioning and tranquilizing action: I. The effect of tranquilizing drugs on conditioned reflex Type II activity following transformation of an alimentary CS into a defense CS: Acta Biologiae Experimentalis 24(1) 1964, 37-45.
  • Wolach, A. H., Breuning, S. E., & Solhkhan, N. (1978). External inhibition in a goldfish (Carassius auratus) classical conditioning situation: Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science Vol 13(2) Apr-Jun 1978, 83-92.
  • Wolgin, D. L., & Jakubow, J. J. (2004). Tolerance to Amphetamine Hypophagia: A Real-Time Depiction of Learning to Suppress Stereotyped Movements in the Rat: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 118(3) Jun 2004, 470-478.
  • Wolgin, D. L., & Wade, J. V. (1995). Learned suppression of stereotypy in amphetamine-treated rats: Implications for understanding tolerance to amphetamine "anorexia." Behavioural Pharmacology Vol 6(3) Apr 1995, 254-262.
  • Worell, L. (1965). The ring of punishment: A theoretical and experimental analogue of repression-suppression: Journal of Abnormal Psychology Vol 70(3) Jun 1965, 201-209.
  • Wright, D. C., & Gustavson, K. K. (1986). Preexposure of the conditioning context and latent inhibition from reduced conditioning: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 24(6) Nov 1986, 451-452.
  • Wright, D. C., Skala, K. D., & Peuser, K. A. (1986). Latent inhibition from context-dependent retrieval of conflicting information: Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society Vol 24(2) Mar 1986, 152-154.
  • Wright, W. G., & Carew, T. J. (1995). A single identified interneuron gates tail-shock induced inhibition in the siphon withdrawal reflex of Aplysia: Journal of Neuroscience Vol 15(1, Pt 2) Jan 1995, 790-797.
  • Wu, M.-f., Krueger, J., Ison, J. R., & Gerrard, R. L. (1984). Startle reflex inhibition in the rat: Its persistence after extended repetition of the inhibitory stimulus: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 10(2) Apr 1984, 221-228.
  • Xie, Z. C., Buckner, E., & Commissaris, R. L. (1995). Anticonflict effect of MK-801 in rats: Time course and chronic treatment studies: Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior Vol 51(4) Aug 1995, 635-640.
  • Yamada, F., Nakayama, M., & Miyata, Y. (1983). Independence of startle elicitation from lead-stimulus inhibition in man: Japanese Journal of Psychology Vol 53(6) Feb 1983, 383-386.
  • Yarczower, M., & Evans, G. (1974). "Combined cue" test of conditioned inhibition in pigeons: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 87(2) Aug 1974, 261-266.
  • Yee, B. K., Feldon, J., & Rawlins, J. N. P. (1995). Latent inhibition in rats is abolished by NMDA-induced neuronal loss in the retrohippocampal region, but this lesion effect can be prevented by systemic haloperidol treatment: Behavioral Neuroscience Vol 109(2) Apr 1995, 227-240.
  • Yeo, A. G. (1974). The acquisition of conditioned suppression as a function of interstimulus interval duration: The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Vol 26(3) Aug 1974, 405-416.
  • Yin, H., Barnet, R. C., & Miller, R. R. (1994). Second-order conditioning and Pavlovian conditioned inhibition: Operational similarities and differences: Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes Vol 20(4) Oct 1994, 419-428.
  • Zalstein-Orda, N., & Lubow, R. E. (1995). Context control of negative transfer induced by preexposure to irrelevant stimuli: Latent inhibition in humans: Learning and Motivation Vol 26(1) Feb 1995, 11-28.
  • Zeri, F., De Luce, M., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2005). Intermittent Central Suppression: An Evaluation of Its Presence Using Three Instruments: Journal of Behavioral Optometry Vol 16(3) Jun 2005, 59-62.
  • Zielinski, K., & Walasek, G. (1977). Stimulus intensity and conditioned suppression magnitude: Dependence upon the type of comparison and stage of training: Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis Vol 37(5) 1977, 299-309.
  • Zolman, J. F., Sahley, C. L., & Hall, J. A. (1976). Ontogeny of conditioned inhibition in domestic chicks: Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Vol 90(12) Dec 1976, 1116-1125.


External links[]

video of conditioned suppression in the rat


Learning
Types of learning
Avoidance conditioning | Classical conditioning | Confidence-based learning | Discrimination learning | Emulation | Experiential learning | Escape conditioning | Incidental learning |Intentional learning | Latent learning | Maze learning | Mastery learning | Mnemonic learning | Nonassociative learning | Nonreversal shift learning | Nonsense syllable learning | Nonverbal learning | Observational learning | Omission training | Operant conditioning | Paired associate learning | Perceptual motor learning | Place conditioning | Probability learning | Rote learning | Reversal shift learning | Second-order conditioning | Sequential learning | Serial anticipation learning | Serial learning | Skill learning | Sidman avoidance conditioning | Social learning | Spatial learning | State dependent learning | Social learning theory | State-dependent learning | Trial and error learning | Verbal learning 
Concepts in learning theory
Chaining | Cognitive hypothesis testing | Conditioning | Conditioned responses | Conditioned stimulus | Conditioned suppression | Constant time delay | Counterconditioning | Covert conditioning | Counterconditioning | Delayed alternation | Delay reduction hypothesis | Discriminative response | Distributed practice |Extinction | Fast mapping | Gagné's hierarchy | Generalization (learning) | Generation effect (learning) | Habits | Habituation | Imitation (learning) | Implicit repetition | Interference (learning) | Interstimulus interval | Intermittent reinforcement | Latent inhibition | Learning schedules | Learning rate | Learning strategies | Massed practice | Modelling | Negative transfer | Overlearning | Practice | Premack principle | Preconditioning | Primacy effect | Primary reinforcement | Principles of learning | Prompting | Punishment | Recall (learning) | Recency effect | Recognition (learning) | Reconstruction (learning) | Reinforcement | Relearning | Rescorla-Wagner model | Response | Reinforcement | Secondary reinforcement | Sensitization | Serial position effect | Serial recall | Shaping | Stimulus | Reinforcement schedule | Spontaneous recovery | State dependent learning | Stimulus control | Stimulus generalization | Transfer of learning | Unconditioned responses | Unconditioned stimulus 
Animal learning
Cat learning | Dog learning  Rat learning 
Neuroanatomy of learning
Neurochemistry of learning
Adenylyl cyclase  
Learning in clinical settings
Applied Behavior Analysis | Behaviour therapy | Behaviour modification | Delay of gratification | CBT | Desensitization | Exposure Therapy | Exposure and response prevention | Flooding | Graded practice | Habituation | Learning disabilities | Reciprocal inhibition therapy | Systematic desensitization | Task analysis | Time out 
Learning in education
Adult learning | Cooperative learning | Constructionist learning | Experiential learning | Foreign language learning | Individualised instruction | Learning ability | Learning disabilities | Learning disorders | Learning Management | Learning styles | Learning theory (education) | Learning through play | School learning | Study habits 
Machine learning
Temporal difference learning | Q-learning 
Philosophical context of learning theory
Behaviourism | Connectionism | Constructivism | Functionalism | Logical positivism | Radical behaviourism 
Prominant workers in Learning Theory|-
Pavlov | Hull | Tolman | Skinner | Bandura | Thorndike | Skinner | Watson 
Miscellaneous|-
Category:Learning journals | Melioration theory 
edit
Advertisement