Assessment |
Biopsychology |
Comparative |
Cognitive |
Developmental |
Language |
Individual differences |
Personality |
Philosophy |
Social |
Methods |
Statistics |
Clinical |
Educational |
Industrial |
Professional items |
World psychology |
Cognitive Psychology: Attention · Decision making · Learning · Judgement · Memory · Motivation · Perception · Reasoning · Thinking - Cognitive processes Cognition - Outline Index
The modality effect is a term used in experimental psychology, most often in the fields dealing with memory and learning, to refer to how learner performance depends on the presentation mode of studied items.
Description[]
Modality can refer to a number of characteristics of the presented study material. However, this term is usually used to describe the improved recall of the final items of a list when that list is presented in an auditory manner in comparison with a visual representation. The effect is seen in free recall (recall of list items in any given order), serial recall (recall of list items in the order of study), short-term sentence recall (recall specific words from sentences with similar meanings) and paired associate recall (recall of a pair from presentation of one of its members). For paired associates, the effect is limited to an increased probability of recall for the final 2 or 3 pairs studied.[1] In free recall and serial recall, the modality effect is seen as simply an exaggerated recency effect in tests where presentation is auditory. In short-term sentence recall studies, emphasis is placed on words in a distractor-word list when requesting information from the remembered sentence. This demonstrates the modality effect can be more than auditory or visual.[2]
For serial recall, the modality effect is seen in an increased memory span for auditorally presented lists. Memory span is defined as the maximum number of items that participants correctly recall on 505 of trials. Typically, studies find these to be seven digits, six letters and five words.[3] In a study done by Drewnowski and Murdock, a visual list of English words was found to have an immediate recall of 4.82 words while an auditory representation of this same list led to a memory span of 5.36, a statistically significant variance.[4]
Some studies use the term modality to refer to a general difference in performance based upon the mode of presentation. For example, Gibbons demonstrated modality effects in an experiment by making participants count either beeping sounds or visually presented dots. The to-be-remembered number was derived from the number of dots or beeps counted.[5] In memory experiments, the modality effect is an example of source clustering, which refers to the tendency of items presented in the same modality to be grouped together during recall.[6] Within-list manipulations of modality affect recall probability, order of recall, and grouping.[7]
Bennet Murdock used a basic free recall paradigm, with different types of lists, mixing auditorally and visually presented words. The results he obtained showed that modality improved recency but did not affect recall for the pre-recency items. This effect was seen to be slightly larger when the items for study were presented more rapidly.[8] However, with mixed list presentations (lists presented both auditorally and visually in a single study period) the superiority of auditory study is seen in all serial positions, not just in recency. Murdock interprets this as evidence for separate short term stores for visual and auditory memory.
Glenberg[9] showed that the modality effect is also prevalent in long term memory, showing that to-be-remembered word pairs that are separated by distractor activity are better recalled if presented auditorally vs. visually. By using techniques similar to Murdock’s free recall paradigm, plus the addition of varied amounts of distraction time (filled with counting backwards), Glenberg showed that the modality effect is not affected by a disruptive task and therefore is theoretically not restricted to short term memory.
Several terms have been used to refer to the modality effect on recency. Crowder and Morton[10] refer to it as PAS, or precategorical acoustic store. This and other similar terms(echoic memory, phonological loop) are used to explain a specialized short-term memory system store for phonological information.[11]
See also[]
References[]
- ↑ Murdock, Bennet B. (1972). Short-term Memory. The psychology of learning and motivation:advances in research and theory 5: 67–127.
- ↑ Goolkasian, Paula, Paul W. Foos, Mirrenda Eaton (2009). Modality Effects in Sentence Recall. The Journal of General Psychology 136 (2): 205–224.
- ↑ Crannell, C.W., Parish, J.M. (1957). A comparison of immediate memory span for digits, letters and words. Journal of Psychology 44: 319–327.
- ↑ Drewnowski, Adam, Murdock, B.B. (1980). The role of auditory features in memory span for words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 6: 319–332.
- ↑ Gibbons, Jeffrey A., Velkey, Andrew K.; Partin, Kathren T. (January 2008). Influence of recall procedures on the modality effect with numbers and enumerated stimuli. Journal of General Psychology 135: 84–104.
- ↑ Kahana, Michael J., Polyn, Sean M.; Norman, Keith A. (2009). A Context Maintenance and Retrieval Model of Organizational Processes in Free Recall. Psychological Review 116: 129–156.
- ↑ Murdock, Benet B., Walker, Keith D. (1969). Modality Effects in Free Recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 8: 665–676.
- ↑ Murdock, Bennett B., Walker, Kenneth D. (1969). Modality effects in free recall. Journal of Verbal Leaning and Verbal Behavior 8: 665–676.
- ↑ Glenberg, Arthur M. (January 1984). A retrieval account of the long-term modality effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 10: 16–31.
- ↑ Crowder, Robert G., Morton, John (1969). Precategorical acoustic storage (PAS). Perception & Psychophysics 5: 365–373.
- ↑ Baddeley, A.D. (1986). Working Memory, Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.
- Neath, I., Surprenant, A.M. (2003). Human memory: An introduction to research, data, and theory, 2nd, Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
External links[]
- The modality effect in multimedia instructions
- A Cognitive Theory of Multimedia Learning: Implications for Design Principles
- Watkins OC, Watkins MJ (September 1980). The modality effect and echoic persistence. J Exp Psychol Gen 109 (3): 251–78.
This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia (view authors). |