Psychology Wiki

Assessment | Biopsychology | Comparative | Cognitive | Developmental | Language | Individual differences | Personality | Philosophy | Social |
Methods | Statistics | Clinical | Educational | Industrial | Professional items | World psychology |

Animals · Animal ethology · Comparative psychology · Animal models · Outline · Index


In evolutionary biology, preadaptation describes a situation where a species evolves to use a preexisting structure or trait inherited from an ancestor for a potentially unrelated function.[1][2] One example of preadaptation is dinosaurs having used feathers for insulation and display before using them to fly, or sweat glands in mammals being transformed into mammary glands.

Another example is the hypothesis proposed by zoologist Jonathan Kingdon that before early humans became bipedal, they began engaging in squat feeding, i.e. turning over rocks and leaves to find insects, worms, snails and other food. Consequently, they adapted flatter feet than were necessary in their previous tree-dwelling ancestors, since that makes squatting much easier. Flatter feet are also extremely useful for bipedal animals, so they can be described as a preadaptation to bipedalism, even though (or rather because) the adaptation had nothing to do with bipedalism originally.

Arthropods provide the earliest identifiable fossils of land animals, from about 419 mllion years ago in the Late Silurian, and terrestrial tracks from about 450million years ago appear to have been made by arthropods.[3] Arthropods were well pre-adapted to colonize land, because their existing jointed exoskeletons provided protection against desiccation, support against gravity and a means of locomotion that was not dependent on water.[4]

Some biologists dislike the term 'preadaptation' as it could imply an intentional plan, which is contrary to the nature of evolution.[5] Some alternative terms that have been suggested include "co-option" and exaptation, to avoid the implication of foresight.

Some phenomena could give the appearance of foresight, however, without actually involving it, being instead attributable to simple probability. For example, future environments (for example, hotter or drier ones), may resemble those already encountered by a population at one of its current spatial or temporal margins.[6] This is not actual foresight, but rather the luck of having adapted to a climate which later becomes more prominent. Cryptic genetic variation may have the most strongly deleterious mutations purged from it, leaving an increased chance of useful adaptations,[7][8] but this represents selection acting on current genomes with consequences for the future, rather than foresight.

See also[]

References[]

  1. Bock, W.J. (1959). Preadaptation and multiple evolutionary pathways. Evolution 13 (2): 194–211.
  2. Hayden, Eric J., Evandro Ferrada, Andreas Wagner (02 June 2011). Cryptic genetic variation promotes rapid evolutionary adaptation in an RNA enzyme. Nature 474 (7349): 92–95.
  3. Pisani, D., Laura L Poling, L.L., Lyons-Weiler M., and Hedges, S.B. (2004). The colonization of land by animals: molecular phylogeny and divergence times among arthropods. BMC Biology 2: 1.
  4. Cowen, R.. History of Life, 3rd, Blackwell Science.
  5. Gould, S.J., Vrba, E.S. (1982). Exaptation - a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology 8: 4–15.
  6. Eshel,I. Matessi, C. (1998). Canalization, genetic assimilation and preadaptation: A quantitative genetic model. Genetics 4: 2119–2133.
  7. Masel, Joanna (March 2006). Cryptic Genetic Variation Is Enriched for Potential Adaptations. Genetics 172 (3): 1985–1991.
  8. Rajon, E., Masel, J. (2011). Evolution of molecular error rates and the consequences for evolvability. PNAS 108 (3): 1082–1087.


This page uses Creative Commons Licensed content from Wikipedia (view authors).